Evaluation of L2 Listening Comprehension: in Pursuit of New Measurement Units

Psycholinguistic concept and methodological tools for systematic description of multidisciplinary subject areas. Development of a unit of measure for determining the grammatical completeness and factual semantics for listening comprehension of English.

Рубрика Иностранные языки и языкознание
Вид статья
Язык английский
Дата добавления 24.09.2023
Размер файла 585,4 K

Отправить свою хорошую работу в базу знаний просто. Используйте форму, расположенную ниже

Студенты, аспиранты, молодые ученые, использующие базу знаний в своей учебе и работе, будут вам очень благодарны.

Размещено на http://allbest.ru

Evaluation of L2 Listening Comprehension: in Pursuit of New Measurement Units

Vitalii Shymko Dr. Sc. in Psychology, Professor

Abstract

Purpose. Second language (L2) listening comprehension remains one of the most under researched problems in field largely due to the corresponding methodological difficulties.

This study focuses on the development of such a measurement unit that considers the actual grammatical completeness and does not distort the factual semantics of the perceived text - listening comprehension unit.

Methods and procedure of research. The concept of listening comprehension unit is developed based on a structural-ontological approach, which is a tool for systemic description of the multidisciplinary subject matter fields. To validate listening comprehension units, a study was carried out on a sample of 38 English L2 learners. Study participants were grouped according to their IELTS results, and listening comprehension unit scores were compared with idea unit ones.

Results. The obtained empirical data were processed with One-way ANOVA test and proper results support the following conclusions. First, listening comprehension units can be used to measure and differentiate levels of L2 listening comprehension of short texts.

Secondly, the study demonstrates a higher differential sensitivity of listening comprehension units compared to idea units in L2 listening comprehension levels diagnostics.

Conclusions. First, listening comprehension units can be used to measure and differentiate levels of L2 listening comprehension of short texts. Secondly, our study demonstrates a higher differential sensitivity of listening comprehension units compared to idea units in L2 listening comprehension levels diagnostics. In our opinion, another advantage of using listening comprehension units is that this tool also allows one to look differently at the semantic structure of the listening comprehension process. In turn, this might open new psycholinguistic, pedagogical and didactic perspectives in the field.

Key words: listening comprehension, psycholinguistic measurement, idea unit, structural-ontological approach, semantic role, subject, actualizer, specification, localization.

Анотація

Оцінка розуміння іноземної мови на слух: У пошуках нових одиниць вимірювання

Віталій Шимко

доктор психологічних наук, професор

Мета. Розуміння на слух іноземної мови залишається однією з найбільш недосліджених проблем у цій галузі значною мірою через відповідні методологічні труднощі. Дослідження спрямоване на розробку такої одиниці вимірювання, яка враховує фактичну граматичну повноту і не спотворює фактичну семантику сприйнятого на слух іноземного тексту - одиниці розуміння сприйнятого на слух.

Методики дослідження. На основі структурно-онтологічного підходу розроблено концепцію одиниць розуміння сприйнятого на слух, який є методологічним інструментом для системного опису мультидисциплінарних предметних галузей.

Щоб підтвердити одиниці розуміння сприйнятого на слух, було проведено дослідження на вибірці з 38 осіб, які вивчають англійську L2. Учасників дослідження об'єднали в групи відповідно до їхніх результатів IELTS, а оцінки одиниць розуміння сприйнятого на слух порівняли з ідейними одиницями.

Результати. Отримані емпіричні дані були оброблені за допомогою тесту One-way ANOVA, й відповідні результати підтверджують наступні висновки.

По-перше, одиниці розуміння сприйнятого на слух можна використовувати для вимірювання та диференціації рівнів розуміння коротких іншомовних текстів. По-друге, дослідження демонструє більш високу диференціальну чутливість одиниць розуміння сприйнятого на слух порівняно з ідейними одиницями в діагностиці рівнів розуміння сприйнятого на слух іншомовного контенту.

Висновки. По-перше, одиниці розуміння сприйнятого на слух можна використовувати для вимірювання та диференціації рівнів розуміння коротких іншомовних текстів.

По-друге, наше дослідження демонструє більш точну диференціальну чутливість одиниць розуміння сприйнятого на слух порівняно з ідейними одиницями в діагностиці рівнів розуміння іншомовних текстів на слух. На нашу думку, ще однією перевагою використання одиниць розуміння сприйнятого на слух є те, що цей інструмент також дозволяє по-іншому поглянути на семантичну структуру процесу розуміння іншомовного контенту. У свою чергу, це може відкрити нові психолінгвістичні, педагогічні та дидактичні перспективи в даній галузі.

Ключові слова: розуміння сприйнятого на слух, іншомовний текст,

психолінгвістичне вимірювання, ідейна одиниця, структурно-онтологічний підхід, семантична роль, суб'єкт, актуалізатор, специфікація, локалізація.

Introduction

Second language (L2) listening comprehension remains one of the most under researched problems in the L2 learning field. Many of the early and modern researchers generally agree on the critical role of the listening comprehension for L2 learning and on methodological difficulties of studying the phenomenon (Chang, 2012; El-dali, 2017; J. Field, 2009; Goh, 2016; Graham et al., 2011; Hansen & Jensen, 1994; Joyce, 2019; Long, 1985; Ockey & Wagner, 2018; Wang & Treffers-Daller, 2017; Yeldham & Gruba, 2016). Aptly pointed out by Yeldham as “most ephemeral of the macro-skills” (Yeldham, 2017), the L2 listening comprehension still remains discussed whether it relates to a language problem or listening problem (El-dali, 2017) students typically have limited exposure to the language outside formal classrooms.

Therefore, their ability to comprehend spoken English may be limited. To add to this problem, L2 learners often regard listening as the most difficult language skill to learn. On the other hand, it is noticeable that L2 listening remains the least researched of all four language skills. Accordingly, the present study is based on the commonly believed premises that (1. This uncertainty is extrapolated to the empirical study domain, leading to a variety of research techniques (Yeldham, 2017), none of which seem to provide a way to a holistic understanding of the L2 listening comprehension subject matter field. Hence the issue of appropriate measurement units is of particular methodological importance and interest. In this regard, there are also gamut of approaches that are used throughout the L2 studies and adjacent research areas for measurement purposes - words (Yang et al., 2020), sentences (Bardovi-Harlig, 1992), propositions (Sato, 1988), clauses (Taboada & Zabala, 2008), T-units (Sotillo, 2000)b, C-units (Pica et al., 1989)but that opportunities for nonnative speakers (NNSs, AS-units (Foster et al., 2000) there must be agreement on the nature of the unit, and it must be possible to apply this unit reliably to a range of different types of speech data. There are a number of different units in use, the various merits of which have been discussed by Crookes (1990, prosodic units (Izre'el et al., 2020), idea units (Richards et al., 2016), discourse units (Houtkoop & Mazeland, 1985), turn-constructional units (Liddicoat, 2004) etc.

These approaches reflect the evading multifaceted nature of the comprehension phenomenon and discussions on this matter are most likely to continue. However, along with the theoretical question of how the comprehension occurs, at the same time, the practical question of what exactly is comprehended in each specific pedagogical, communicative or interactive situation continues to remain relevant. This is especially important at the more advanced stages of language acquisition, when the challenge of the L2 grammatical complexity mastering somewhat recedes into the background and further growth is more and more associated with the cognitive sensitivity of students to the semantic nuances of the language, the ability to understand subtle humor, puns and take into account many linguacultural factors. In our opinion, the possible solution to the problem lies beyond a predominantly linguistic model of T-units (Bardovi-Harlig, 1992; Gaies, 1980; Hunt, 1977; Sotillo, 2000; Taboada & Zabala, 2008) the sentence-based analysis is superior for examining syntactic complexity in at least three ways: (1 towards the idea units concept, founded mostly on a cognitive base (Cocklin et al., 1984; Dunlosky et al., 2011; Granaas, 1985; Kroll, 1977; Vandergrift & Goh, 2011; Yeldham & Gruba, 2016). Syntactic emphasis in the process of identifying T-unit might “suppress” or involuntary “correct” some implied semantics, which seems counterproductive in the situation of assessing listening comprehension. On the other hand, an idea unit might “omit” or “misread” grammatical elements. Thus, such a measurement unit is needed that takes into account the actual grammatical completeness and does not distort the factual semantics of the perceived text. The main goal of this article is to present results of our corresponding attempt to create a new technique for assessing L2 listening comprehension.

Materials and Methods

To develop a theoretical model of the above technique, we proceeded from the following definition of an idea unit: “chunk of information corresponding to a cognitive or psychological reality of the speaker” (Kroll, 1977). It is obvious that the structural unit of this reality is a semantic one. How might such a semantic unit be organized? To answer the question we used a structural-ontological approach (Shymko, 2019b).

The approach has been developed and probed in some research scopes (philosophy of mind and AI, psycholinguistics, organizational studies) as a methodological tool for a systemic description of the investigated multidisciplinary subject matter field (Shymko, 2018, 2019a, 2019c; Shymko & Babadzhanova, 2020).

The description involves a construction of a structural-ontological matrix that reflects the system-forming factors (i.e., a primary process and a material of the system under the study) and allows one to conceptualize theoretical hypotheses and appropriately build up an empirical research architecture.

Here we omit the relevant procedural aspects, which are detailed in the publications mentioned above, and move on to the visualization directly related to this study (Fig. 1).

We considered a listening comprehension unit (LC unit) as the system with its material component as a subject (the horizontal axis of the matrix, Fig. 1). The subject can be any entity: animate or inanimate, human or organizational, tangible or abstract, singular or groupwise, natural or artificial, real or imaginary, well defined or vague, specific or implied, etc.

The subject is who or what is being talked about in each particular fragment of the message. Also, the subject is the author of the direct or quoted speech. The subject can appear alone in a text fragment or have some characteristics (in the broadest possible sense - have any properties and peculiarities, carry out or undergo some actions, have certain experiences, expectations, intentions, origin, focus, be somehow located in space and time, etc.). Typically, the subject is a noun or pronoun and may consist of one or more words.

Figure 1

The Structural-Ontological Matrix of a L2 Listening Comprehension Unit

The subject can be the name of an organization, a field or type of activity, any kind of situation or state of affairs, etc. Some characteristics of one subject can be detected in semantic interaction with another one. In this case, the latter subject acts as a semantic actualizer of the characteristics of the former one.

For example, in the sentence: “She described the security operation underway in a city”, the subject is “she”, and the actualizer is “security operation”. Note that a subject in one fragment can become an actualizer in another fragment and vice versa.

Primarily, the textual size of the listening comprehension unit is determined by one subject and one actualizer. So, if the properties of one subject are revealed in the text by two or more actualizers, it is necessary to multiply the number of the units accordingly.

As the primary process of the above-mentioned system, we considered the semantic unfolding of the subject and the actualizer (vertical axis) in the text fragment. Such unfolding, on the one hand, presupposes endowing the subject and the actualizer with various properties and characteristics, as well as reporting on their activity.

We categorized the listed semantic parameters as - specification. On the other hand, the subject and the actualizer can be revealed in a text fragment as having spatial and/or temporal characteristics, which we classified as - localization. Specifiers and localizers also take part in sizing the unit.

So if a text fragment is replete with them, then we consider two possibilities: (a) we leave synonymously close specifiers or localizers within one unit (see the example in line 32, Table A1); (b) in other cases, we form a new unit, in which, when calculating points, we do not take into account the subject, actualizer as well as previously taken into account specifiers and localizers (see the example in rows 12-13, Table A1).

Same as the last step we solve the anaphora problem. We consider a unit as such that can be complete (i.e., it can consist of a subject, an actualizer and their specifiers and localizers) or partial. It might comprise only one out of the six components of the structural- ontological matrix (Fig. 1) if such unit is semantically autonomous in the text.

For example, in: “Twilight. It is getting dark all around the valley” we have two units. First one - “twilight” (consist of the only subject). And in the second unit we have as a subject - “it”, specifier - “getting dark”, localizer - “all around the valley”.

When calculating results, each structural component of a unit corresponds to one point. Thus, the maximum possible number of points for one unit is six. We believe that the convergence of the structural components of the unit constitutes semantic roles (Donohue & Wichmann, 2008) of the subject and the actualizer (Fig. 1). A recognition of the roles is an important qualitative indicator of listening comprehension.

Therefore, semantic role errors were considered as gross and on a par with omissions, they were assessed with zero points. In other cases of non-critical inaccuracies in the content comprehension, we estimated the corresponding component of the unit at half a point if the inaccuracies did not distort the fragment meaning essentially. To validate the described technique, we needed to answer two research questions:

RQ 1. Does this technique measure and differentiate levels of L2 listening comprehension?

RQ 2. How effective is the use of proposed listening comprehension units compared to idea units?

In order to address above questions, we conducted a study. The participants were 38 female and 7 male undergraduate and graduate students (with different learning period: from 4 to 8 semesters) majoring English Philology and Translation in Department of Foreign Philology, Translation and Teaching Methods at Hryhorii Skovoroda University in

Pereiaslav, Ukraine. All of the students were Ukrainian native speakers. The study was conducted two-three weeks after the participants passed the annual IELTS exam.

Based on the IELTS listening band score level, the participants were divided into three groups: B1, B2 and C1. For such differentiation some benchmarking results were taken into account (Hidri, 2020).

An excerpt from one of the issues of BBC Global News Podcast (Miles, 2021) was used to collect the necessary data. The duration of the excerpt was 1 minute 23 seconds.

Students were instructed to reproduce the content they heard as completely and accurately as possible and were warned to avoid paraphrasing as well as their own contributions, comments, speculations, etc. After that, they listened to the excerpt once, during which they were allowed to take notes. Verbal reports (Yeldham, 2017) of the participants were registered immediately after listening using the protocol based on a table with the excerpt transcription parsed into listening comprehension units (Appendix A).

When it was necessary, the experimenters asked clarifying questions in order to make sure that semantic roles of subjects and actualizers were correctly understood. Each structural component of every listening comprehension unit was estimated at 1 point if it was accurately reproduced in the verbal report. If there were insignificant errors, the score was reduced to 0.5 points. Significant errors, omissions and appearance of irrelevant semantics were scored 0 points.

In addition to the above, the participants' verbal reports were processed and evaluated using another protocol for calculating idea units scores (the breakdown into these units can also be found in Appendix A).

For this purpose, the following rating system was used:

3 points - the content is reproduced accurately, completely;

2 point - the content is reproduced either somewhat incompletely or with insignificant distortions of semantics;

1 point - gross errors in understanding were made and the content of the unit was reproduced only partially to the detriment of unit comprehension;

0 points - content omitted or substituted by irrelevant one. Statistical analysis of obtained results was done using IBM SPSS Statistics V26.

All materials, data, audio recordings, transcriptions and protocols associated with this study are in open access availability on Harvard Dataverse Repository (Name deleted to maintain the integrity of the review process).

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Normality Tests

The sample as a whole was relatively young (M = 21.42, SD = 1.97) and predominantly female (81.6%). Scored points on the excerpt listening comprehension were tested for normality (Fig. 2).

listening comprehension grammatical english

Figure 2

Normal Q-Q Plots of Scores Distribution

In both cases, the data is normally distributed. Along with the graphs, this is evidenced by parameters of Skewness (- .169 for idea units and .089 for LC units) and Kurtosis (- 1.251 and - 1.240, respectively).

Also, the null hypothesis of the normality test is confirmed by the Kolmogorov-Smirnov criterion (for idea units p = .146 and for LC units p = .200).

In accordance with the results of the IELTS, the B1 group included 12 students (31.6%), and the B2 and C1 groups each included 13 students (34.2%). The corresponding descriptive statistic are given in the table 1.

Table 1

Descriptives for Idea Units and LC units by IELTS Groups.

95% Confidence Interval for Mean

N

Mean

Std.Deviation

Std.Error

Lower Bound

Upper Bound

Minimum

Maximum

Idea

B1

12

70.50

5.436

1.569

67.05

73.95

64

79

units

B2

13

79.77

4.885

1.355

76.82

82.72

72

89

C1

13

86.08

9.269

2.571

80.48

91.68

65

93

Total

38

79.00

9.253

1.501

75.96

82.04

64

93

LC

B1

12

75.08

4.481

1.294

72.24

77.93

70

84

units

B2

13

89.15

3.211

.890

87.21

91.09

85

95

C1

13

98.92

11.214

3.110

92.15

105.70

74

109

Total

38

88.05

12.108

1.964

84.07

92.03

70

109

One-Way ANOVA Test

A one-way analysis of variance was conducted to evaluate the null hypothesis that there is no difference of listening comprehension measurements using idea units and LC units based on L2 learners IELTS scores. The assumption of normality for both units was proven above.

Idea Units

The assumption of homogeneity of variances was tested and found tenable using Levene's test F (2, 35) = 1.224, p = .306. The ANOVA was significant F (2, 35) = 16.259, p < .001. Thus, there is ground to reject the null hypothesis and conclude there is a significant difference of listening comprehension measurements using idea units based on L2 learners IELTS scores. Post hoc comparison (Table 2) to evaluate pairwise differences among group means were conducted with the use of Tukey HSD test since equal variances were tenable.

Table 2

Multiple Comparisons (Idea Units)

95% Confidence Interval

Dependent

Variable

(I)

IELTS

(J)

IELTS

Mean

Difference (I-J)

Std.

Error

Sig.

Lower Upper

Bound Bound

Idea units

B1

B2

-9.269*

2.742

.005

-15.98

-2.56

C1

-15.577*

2.742

.000

-22.29

-8.87

B2

B1

9.269*

2.742

.005

2.56

15.98

C1

-6.308

2.687

.062

-12.88

.27

C1

B1

15.577*

2.742

.000

8.87

22.29

B2

6.308

2.687

.062

-.27

12.88

*. The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level

As can be seen, tests revealed significant pairwise differences between the mean scores of L2 learners listening comprehension measurements for B1 IELTS group and idea unit measurements for B2 and C1 students, p < .005.

However, measurements in these two latter groups do not significantly differ from each other, p > .05.

LC Units

The assumption of homogeneity of variances was violated according to Levene's test result F (2, 35) = 3.534, p = .040.

Thus, we used alternative F statistics for determining significance (A. Field, 2013). Welch's F (2, 20.61) = 48.566, p < .001. Games-Howell post hoc procedure was used since the homogeneity of variance assumption was not met.

The results (Table 3) prove significant pairwise differences between the mean scores of L2 learners listening comprehension measurements for each of the three groups, p < .05.

Table 3

Multiple Comparisons (LC Units)

Mean

95% Confidence Interval

Dependent

Variable

(I)

IELTS

(J)

IELTS

Difference

(I-J)

Std. Error

Sig.

Lower Bound

Upper Bound

LC units

B1

B2

-14.071*

1.571

.000

-18.05

-10.09

C1

-23.840*

3.368

.000

-32.53

-15.15

B2

B1

14.071*

1.571

.000

10.09

18.05

C1

-9.769*

3.235

.023

-18.24

-1.30

C1

B1

23.840*

3.368

.000

15.15

32.53

B2

9.769*

3.235

.023

1.30

18.24

*. The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level

Discussion and Conclusions

Anticipating a discussion of the results obtained, we would like to focus on the most essential limitations of our study. These are the quantitative and qualitative characteristics of the sample. For more confident conclusions regarding the validity of LC units, it is necessary not only to increase the number of participants, but also to diversify the sample in a balanced manner according to such criteria as gender, age, duration of L2 learning, frequency and volume of texts listening, etc. It is as well necessary to further study the effectiveness of LC units on longer audio texts and in situations of real communication. It would also be informative to compare the effectiveness of LC units not only with idea units, but also with T-units and other instruments of L2 listening comprehension evaluation. Of particular interest is the validity of LC units for students from various ethnolinguistic groups who study various foreign languages.

Returning to the research questions and considering the limitations outlined above, we believe it reasonable to draw the following tentative conclusions. First, LC units can be used to measure and differentiate levels of L2 listening comprehension of short texts. Secondly, our study demonstrates a higher differential sensitivity of LC units compared to idea units in L2 listening comprehension levels diagnostics. In our opinion, another advantage of using LC units is that this tool also allows one to look differently at the semantic structure of the listening comprehension process. In turn, this might open new psycholinguistic, pedagogical and didactic perspectives in the field.

Adherence to ethical standards. Ethics Declarations. Prior to the start of the study, informed consent was obtained from all participants for voluntary participation. The study was conducted in compliance with domestic (Ethical code, 2009) and international ethical standards (American Psychological Association, 2002). The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by Ethics Committee of Hryhorii Skovoroda University in Pereiaslav.

Funding. This research received no external funding.

Informed Consent Statement. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement. The data presented in this study are openly available in Harvard Dataverse at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/IAMSOI

Conflicts of Interest. The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Author contribution statement. The author is the only person that contributed to all parts of this paper.

Consent for publication. The author approve of this submission and, conditional upon the decision made by the editorial board from the peer-review process, consent to the publication of the current work. The work has not been submitted to other journals in consideration for publication.

Open Access. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (CC BY 4.0).

References

Bardovi-Harlig, K. (1992). A Second Look at T-Unit Analysis: Reconsidering the Sentence. TESOL Quarterly, 26(2), 390. https://doi.org/10.2307/3587016

Chang, S. (2012). Approaching L2 Listening Comprehension for Advanced Learners: Using Reading as a Pre-Listening Task. The Korean Language in America, 17, 166-186. https://www.jstor.org/stable/42922364

Cocklin, T.G., Ward, N.J., Chen, H.C., & Juola, J.F. (1984). Factors influencing readability of rapidly presented text segments. Memory & Cognition, 12(5), 431442. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198304

Donohue, M., & Wichmann, S. (Eds.). (2008). The Typology of Semantic

Alignment. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/ACPROF:O SO/9780199238385.001.0001

Dunlosky, J., Hartwig, M.K., Rawson, K.A., & Lipko, A.R. (2011). Improving college students' evaluation of text learning using idea-unit standards. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(3), 467-484. https://doi.org/10.1080/174 70218.2010.502239

El-Dali, M. (2017). L2 Listening Comprehension: Is it a Language Problem or Listening Problem? English Linguistics Research, 6(1), 14-37. https://doi. org/10.5430/ELR.V6N1P14

Field, A. (2013). Discovering Statistics using IBM SPSS Statistics. Sage Publications Ltd. SAGE. https://books.google.com/books/about/Discovering_Statistics_Using_ IBM_SPSS_St.html?id=c0Wk9IuBmAoC

Field, J. (2009). Listening in the Language Classroom. In Listening in the Language Classroom. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/ CBO9780511575945

Foster, P., Tonkyn, A., & Wigglesworth, G. (2000). Measuring spoken language: a unit for all reasons. Applied Linguistics, 21(3), 354-375. https://doi.org/10.1093/ APPLIN/21.3.354

Gaies, S.J. (1980). T-Unit Analysis in Second Language Research: Applications, Problems and Limitations. TESOL Quarterly, 14(1), 50-60. https://doi.org/10.2307/3586808 Goh, C. (2016). Cognition, Metacognition, and L2 Listening. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook of Research in Second Language Teaching and Learning (1st ed., pp. 214-228). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315716893-16 Graham, S., Santos, D., & Vanderplank, R. (2011). Exploring the relationship between listening development and strategy use. Language Teaching Research, 15(4), 435-456. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168811412026 Granaas, M.M. (1985). Simple, applied text parsing. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 17(2), 209-216. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03214385 Hansen, C., & Jensen, C. (1994). Evaluating lecture comprehension. In J. Flowerdew (Ed.), Academic listening (pp. 241-268). Cambridge University Press. https://doi. org/10.1017/CBO9781139524612.019

Hidri, S. (2020). The IELCA and IELTS Exams: A Benchmark Report. The Journal of Asia TEFL, 17(2), 742-749. https://doi.org/10.18823/ASIATEFL.2020.17.2.33.742 Houtkoop, H., & Mazeland, H. (1985). TURNS AND DISCOURSE UNITS IN EVERYDAY CONVERSATION. Journal of Pragmatics, 9, 595-619. https://doi. org/10.1016/0378-2166(85)90055-4

Hunt, K.W. (1977). Early Blooming and Late Blooming Syntactic Structures. In C.L. Cooper, & L. Odell (Eds.), Evaluating Writing: Describing, Measuring, Judging (pp. 100-114). National Council of Teachers of English.

Izre'el, S., Mello, H., Panunzi, A., & Raso, T. (Eds.). (2020). In Search of Basic Units of Spoken Language. John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi. org/10.1075/SCL.94

Joyce, P. (2019). The Relationship between L2 Listening Proficiency and L2 Aural Language Processing. Journal of Language Teaching and Learning in Thailand, 57, 9-32. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1224420.pdf Kroll, B. (1977). Combining ideas in written and spoken English: A look at subordination and coordination. In E.O. Keenan, & T.L. Bennett (Eds.), Discourse across time and space (pp. 69-108). University of Southern California.

Liddicoat, A.J. (2004). The projectability of turn constructional units and the role of prediction in listening. Discourse Studies, 6(4), 449-469. https://doi. org/10.1177/1461445604046589

Long, M. (1985). Input and Second Language Acquisition Theory. In S. Gass, & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in Second Language Acquisition (pp. 377-393). Newbury House. https://www.scribd.com/doc/179333902/Long-1985-Input-and- Second-Language-Acquisition-Theory-pdf

Miles, N. (host). (2021). Putin and Biden hold talks in Geneva [Audio podcast episode]. In Global News Podcast. BBC World Service. https://www.bbc.co.uk/ programmes/p09lnjvh

Ockey, G.J., & Wagner, E. (2018). Assessing L2 listening: Moving towards authenticity. John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://books.google.com/books/about/ Assessing_L2_Listening.html?id=rDtlDwAAQBAJ Pica, T., Holliday, L., Lewis, N., & Morgenthaler, L. (1989). Comprehensible Output as an Outcome of Linguistic Demands on the Learner. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 11(1), 63-90. https://doi.org/10.1017/S027226310000783X

Richards, T., Peverly, S., Wolf, A., Abbott, R., Tanimoto, S., Thompson, R.,

Nagy, W., & Berninger, V (2016). Idea units in notes and summaries for read texts by keyboard and pencil in middle childhood students with specific learning disabilities: Cognitive and brain findings. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 5(3), 146-155. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TINE.2016.07.005

Sato, C.J. (1988). Origins of Complex Syntax in Interlanguage Development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 10(3), 371-395. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S027226310000749X

Shymko, V. (2018). Object field of organizational culture: methodological

conceptualization. International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 26(4), 602613. https://doi.org/10.n08/ijoa-03-2017-n35

Shymko, V. (2019a). Natural Language Understanding: Methodological Conceptualization. Psycholinguistics, 25(1), 431-443. https://doi.

org/10.31470/2309-1797-2019-25-1-431-443

Shymko, V. (2019b). Introduction to Structural-Ontological Methodology: Analysis of the Subject Matter Field of Personality Socialization). SSRN Electronic Journal. https://doi.org/10.2139/SSRN.3575800

Shymko, V. (2019c). On the Question of the Place and Role of Language in the Process of Personality Socialization: Structural-Ontological Sketch. Psycholinguistics, 26(1), 385-400. https://doi.org/10.31470/2309-1797-2019-26-1-385-400

Shymko, V., & Babadzhanova, A. (2020). Space as a Semantic Unit of a Language Consciousness. Psycholinguistics, 27(1), 335-350. https://doi.org/10.31470/2309- 1797-2020-27-1-335-350

Sotillo, S.M. (2000). Discourse functions and syntactic complexity in synchronous and asynchronous communication. Language Learning & Technology, 4(1), 77-110. http://llt.msu.edu/vol4num1/sotillo/default.html

Taboada, M., & Zabala, L.H. (2008). Deciding on units of analysis within Centering Theory. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 4(1), 63-108. https://doi. org/10.1515/CLLT.2008.003

Vandergrift, L., & Goh, C.C.M. (2011). Teaching and learning second language listening: Metacognition in action (1st ed.). Routledge. https://doi.

org/10.4324/9780203843376

Wang, Y., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2017). Explaining listening comprehension among L2 learners of English: The contribution of general language proficiency, vocabulary knowledge and metacognitive awareness. System, 65, 139-150. https://doi. org/10.1016/J.SYSTEM.2016.12.013

Yang, J., Cai, Q., & Tian, X. (2020). How Do We Segment Text? Two-Stage Chunking Operation in Reading. ENeuro, 7(3), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1523/ ENEURO.0425-19.2020

Yeldham, M. (2017). Techniques for researching L2 listeners. System, 66(December), 13-26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2017.03.001

Yeldham, M., & Gruba, P. (2016). The development of individual learners in an L2 listening strategies course. Language Teaching Research, 20(1), 9-34. https://doi. org/10.1177/1362168814541723

Notes

1 When calculating points, the correctness of the semantic role assessment of the subject is considered if appropriate (agent, experiencer, patient, theme, instrument, purpose, manner etc.). The presence of insignificant inaccuracies is estimated at half a point, with significant errors or omission of the subject - zero.

2 The evaluation of the actualizer is carried out in the same way as the evaluation of the subject.

3 In this case, the mentioning of the century characterizes the properties of the subject (old, historical, from a past culture, etc.), and does not reflect its current localization.

4 Hereinafter, in any anaphora case, the corresponding antecedent is taken into account only once when calculating points.

5 Here localization takes place in the expected future.

6 I.e., recently (time localization).

7 The possessive pronoun serves as a specifier indicating the characteristics of the subject's belonging to the organization.

8 The actualizer is implied one here, i.e., it is the unclarified situation that preceded `our' arrival in the studio.

9 In this case, the subject does not coincide with the actualizer, as it might seem at first glance. Here the subject is the “conscious self', which reflects on ignorance of number of obstacles. That is, “ignorant self' is the actualizer.

10 Despite the obvious temporal semantics of the word `morning', here it is categorized as a specifier since it refers to the word `coffee' and rather answers the question "what kind of coffee?" than render time localization of the particular coffee drinking.

11 Here, unlike other cases, the subject is not just omitted, it is ambiguous one. It is unclear for whom exactly the `unfortune” - for Imogen or Eytif? Participants' awareness of this ambiguity was considered and scored by us during the assessment of the completeness of this fragment comprehension.

12 The absence or lack of something we consider as a specification as well.

13 Localization of what is happening here and now.

14 The actualizer here is a purported reason why `it' (Geneva) was chosen to host the summit. The reason is revealed in the next LC unit.

15 We considered the semantic differences between 'very quickly ' and "at a short notice' as insignificant against the backdrop of the obvious strong synonymous relationship. For these reasons, we decided not to divide this LC unit into two separate ones. However, when assessing the comprehension of the corresponding fragment, we took into account the completeness of these synonyms representation by the study participants.

16 Direct time localization.

Appendix A

Table A1

Audio Excerpt Transcription Parsing into Listening Comprehension Units

AUDIO EXCERPT TRANSCRIPTION From 0.59 to 2.22 (Г23”)

IDEA UNITS

LC units

SCORES for LC units (max.)

SUBJECT1

Specification of subject

Localization of subject

ACTUALIZER2

Specification of actualizer

Localization of actualizer

An 18th century Villa with pastel blue shutters and views across Lake Geneva in Switzerland. It is a serene setting.

1.

18th century Villa

Villa

18th century3

2

2.

Pastel blue shutters

Shutters

Pastel blue

2

3.

Geneva Lake views

Views

Geneva Lake

Switzerland

3

4.

Serene setting

it

Setting

Serene

3

But as we record this podcast, the atmosphere between two of the world's most powerful men who are meeting there is likely to be anything but placid.

5.

Podcast recording

We

Record

Podcast

3

6.

World's powerful men

Two men

Most powerful

World

3

7.

The meeting

-- // ---

Meeting

There

2

8.

Unplacid atmosphere

Atmosphere

Anything but placid

likely to be5

3

President Biden fresh from his Alliance building G7 and NATO meetings, is sitting down for talks with a man who in the past is called a killer - Vladimir Putin.

9.

Alliance building

President Biden

Building

Fresh from6

Alliance

Meetings

G7 and NATO

6

10.

Biden-Putin talks

// --

Sitting down for talks

Man

2

11.

One called a killer

Putin

Called a killer

In the past

3

Washington takes issue with Moscow's record on human rights as well as alleged interference in its elections.

12.

Human rights

Washington

Takes issue

Moscow

Human rights record

4

13.

Election interference

// --

// --

-- // --

Election

interference

Its (US) elections

2

Imogen Foulkes is our Geneva correspondent. And just before we came into the studio, she described the security operation underway in a city that's well known for hosting high level summit.

14.

Geneva correspondent

Imogen Foulkes

Our7

Correspondent

Geneva

4

15.

Before the studio

We

Came into

Studio

?8

Before

4

16.

Security operation

She

Described

Security operation

Underway

City (Geneva)

5

17.

Well known city

City

Well known for

2

18.

High level summit host

//

Hosting

Summit

High level

3

We've got helicopters, we've got tanks, we've got police everywhere.

19.

Helicopters

We

Have got

Helicopters

3

20.

Tanks

//

-- // --

Tanks

1

21.

Police

//

-- // --

Police

Everywhere

2

I mean, just coming to the press center this morning, I don't know how many multiple barriers we went through walking because public transport to stop.

22.

Coming to press center

We

Coming to

This morning

Press center

4

23.

Through barriers

//

Went through

Multiple barriers

2

24.

Walk by foot

//

Walking

Public transport

To stop

3

25.

Reflection of unknowingness

I

Mean

p

Don't know how many

Barriers

5

I was talking to police who've

26.

Talking to police

//

Talking to

Police

Been drafted

Switzerland

4

been drafted in from all over Switzerland, and one lovely Geneva old lady Eytif. She's a day looking to have her morning cup of coffee by the

27.

Talking to lady

//

//

Eytif

Lovely old lady

Geneva

3

28.

Morning coffee by the lake

She

Looking to have

Day

Cup of coffee

Morning10

By the lake

6

lake. And unfortunately, no chance today.

29.

Unfortunate chance

For whom?11

Unfortunately

Chance

There is no12

Today

5

But as you say, Geneva is used to this. It was probably chosen because it can stage something this huge, very, very quickly at short notice. And that's what's happening in a couple of days. It'll all be back to normal, I think.

30.

Geneva's habit

You

Say

Geneva

Is used to

This13

5

31

Choice of Geneva

It

Was chosen

Because.. ,14

3

32.

To stage fast and huge

It

Can stage very quickly at short notice15

Something

This huge

4

33

For a couple days

That

Happening

Couple days

3

34

Back to normal

I

Think

Be back to Will16 normal

5

Notes:

1 When calculating points, the correctness of the semantic role assessment of the subject is considered if appropriate (agent, experiencer, patient, theme, instrument, purpose, manner etc.). The presence of insigniВcant inaccuracies is estimated at half a point, with significant errors or omission of the subject - zero.

2 The evaluation of the actualizer is carried out in the same way as the evaluation of the subject.

3 In this case, the mentioning of the century characterizes the properties of the subject (old, historical, from a past culture, etc.), and does not reflect its current localization.

4 Herein after, in any anaphora case, the corresponding antecedent is taken into account only once when calculating points.

5 Here localization takes place in the expected future.

6 I.e., recently (time localization).

7 The possessive pronoun serves as a speciВer indicating the characteristics of the subject's belonging to the organization.

8 The actualizer is implied one here, i.e., it is the unclariВed situation that preceded `our' arrival in the studio.

9 In this case, the subject does not coincide with the actualizer, as it might seem at first glance. Here the subject is the “conscious self”, which reflects on ignorance of number of obstacles. That is, “ignorant self” is the actualizer.

10 Despite the obvious temporal semantics of the word `morning', here it is categorized as a specifier since it refers to the word `coffee' and rather answers the question "what kind of coffee?" than render time localization of the particular coffee drinking.

11 Here, unlike other cases, the subject is not just omitted, it is ambiguous one. It is unclear for whom exactly the `unfortune” - for Imogen or Eytif? Participants` awareness of this ambiguity was considered and scored by us during the assessment of the completeness of this fragment comprehension.

12 The absence or lack of something we consider as a specification as well.

13 Localization of what is happening here and now.

14 The actualizer here is a purported reason why `it' (Geneva) was chosen to host the summit. The reason is revealed in the next LC unit.

15 We considered the semantic differences between 'very quickly ' and "at a short notice' as insignificant against the backdrop of the obvious strong synonymous relationship. For these reasons, we decided not to divide this LC unit into two separate ones. However, when assessing the comprehension of the corresponding fragment, we took into account the completeness of these synonyms representation by the study participants.

16 Direct time localization.

Размещено на Allbest.ru

...

Подобные документы

  • The principles of teaching listening comprehension. The purpose and nature of the listening comprehension programme. The structure of listening comprehension and types of activities. Listening comprehension tests. The usage of the listening material.

    курсовая работа [40,3 K], добавлен 16.04.2011

  • Teaching listening comprehension as a part of educational process at school. Teacher's speech as a basic form of teaching listening comprehension. Principles for developing listening ability. The use of activities developing listening comprehension.

    курсовая работа [5,4 M], добавлен 31.10.2013

  • Conditions of effective communication. Pronouncing consonants and vowels: Sound/spelling correspondence. Vocabulary and lexical stress patterns. Areas of intersection of Pronunciation with morphology and syntax. Listening for reduced speech features.

    презентация [2,4 M], добавлен 23.10.2012

  • The sources of origin of phraseological units in modern English. Borrowing in the foreign language form. Phraseological units, reflecting the traditions, customs of the English people. Phraseological units connected with beliefs, taken from fairy tales.

    статья [19,1 K], добавлен 03.12.2015

  • The meaning of the term "phraseological unit" in modern linguistics. Characteristics of the national-cultural specifics of phraseological units. The internal forms of phraseological units with an integral part of the name of clothing in English.

    курсовая работа [50,4 K], добавлен 29.10.2021

  • Memory, Teaching and his types. Why we need teaching of memory. Short-term and protracted memory: oppositions and coincidences. Short-term memory and methods of his improvement. Listening of methods is in translation. Scholars of research of listening.

    дипломная работа [45,4 K], добавлен 10.07.2009

  • Concept and features of the Middle English, stages and directions of its formation and development. Primary and Middle English consonants, the basic principles of articles and declination. Personal pronouns, verbs, syntax, semantics and dialects.

    презентация [380,6 K], добавлен 24.04.2014

  • Teaching Vocabulary in English Language: effective Methodologies. Patterns of Difficulty in Vocabulary. Introduction of the Vocabulary. Ways of Determining the Vocabulary Comprehension and Remembering. Key Strategies in Teaching Vocabulary.

    курсовая работа [204,1 K], добавлен 06.12.2015

  • The concept and category values "voice" and "mood" in different languages. Features and comparative description of the use and formation of a voice in English and Armenian. Classification of moods, their characteristics of a grammatical point of view.

    курсовая работа [43,1 K], добавлен 06.10.2015

  • The grammatical units consisting of one or more words that bear minimal syntactic relation to the words that precede or follow it. Pragmatic word usage. Differences in meaning. Idioms and miscommunications. The pragmatic values of evidential sentences.

    статья [35,2 K], добавлен 18.11.2013

  • The term "concept" in various fields of linguistics. Metaphor as a language unit. The problem of defining metaphor. The theory of concept. The notion of concept in Linguistics. Metaphoric representation of the concept "beauty" in English proverbs.

    курсовая работа [22,2 K], добавлен 27.06.2011

  • Contextual and functional features of the passive forms of grammar in English. Description of the rules of the time in the passive voice. Principles of their translation into Russian. The study of grammatical semantics combinations to be + Participle II.

    курсовая работа [51,9 K], добавлен 26.03.2011

  • Grammatical overview of English verbals. General characteristics of English verbals. General characteristics of Participles. Syntax and Semantics of English Verbals. The functions of the Infinitive in the sentence. Syntax and semantics of participles.

    дипломная работа [72,9 K], добавлен 10.07.2009

  • The concept as the significance and fundamental conception of cognitive linguistics. The problem of the definition between the concept and the significance. The use of animalism to the concept BIRD in English idioms and in Ukrainian phraseological units.

    курсовая работа [42,0 K], добавлен 30.05.2012

  • Investigation on the eventual change of setting of Shakespeare’s play Measure for Measure. Play’s main points are lechery, hypocrisy, all what was associated with the Italy of that time. This play - English play written before 1660 that is set in Vienna.

    реферат [60,1 K], добавлен 20.12.2010

  • The existent problems in teaching reading comprehension and finding the ways out of this problem by suggesting the exercises that can be useful in classroom activities. The reading skills and teaching technics, new technologies in teaching reading.

    курсовая работа [23,9 K], добавлен 17.04.2011

  • The notion of the grammatical category of gender. The main approaches in investigating the category of gender, the ways of expressing in English and Uzbek. Zoonims as separate lexical units. Generic categorization of zoonims in English and Uzbek.

    курсовая работа [79,3 K], добавлен 05.04.2013

  • A word-group as the largest two-facet lexical unit. The aptness of a word, its lexical and grammatical valency. The lexical valency of correlated words in different languages. Morphological motivation as a relationship between morphemic structure.

    контрольная работа [17,4 K], добавлен 09.11.2010

  • The development of American English pronunciation. English changes in which most North American dialects do not participate. Eastern and Southern American English. Australian speech as a subject to debate. Long and short vowels. Canadian pronunciation.

    реферат [62,2 K], добавлен 14.05.2011

  • A critical knowledge of the English language is a subject worthy of the attention of all who have the genius and the opportunity to attain it. A settled orthography is of great importance, as a means of preserving the etymology and identity of words.

    курсовая работа [28,1 K], добавлен 14.02.2010

Работы в архивах красиво оформлены согласно требованиям ВУЗов и содержат рисунки, диаграммы, формулы и т.д.
PPT, PPTX и PDF-файлы представлены только в архивах.
Рекомендуем скачать работу.