Communicative failures caused by ethological factors (on the material of American cinema discourse)
Identifying communicative failures that are the result of not realizing ethical norms of behavior. Application of inferential analysis to study of examples simulating live communication in the American cinematic discourse represented by the comedy genre.
Рубрика | Культура и искусство |
Вид | статья |
Язык | английский |
Дата добавления | 16.08.2022 |
Размер файла | 28,2 K |
Отправить свою хорошую работу в базу знаний просто. Используйте форму, расположенную ниже
Студенты, аспиранты, молодые ученые, использующие базу знаний в своей учебе и работе, будут вам очень благодарны.
Размещено на http://allbest.ru
Simon Kuznets Kharkiv National University of Economics
Communicative failures caused by ethological factors (on the material of American cinema discourse)
Dubtsova O. V. Ph.D. in Philology, Associate Professor
at the Department of Pedagogy, Foreign Philology and Translation
Kharkiv, Ukraine
Abstract
The paper reveals and describes communicative failures caused by differences in structures of communicants' ethological encyclopaedic knowledge within the framework of the intersubjectivity paradigm. A communicative failure is viewed as an inability of interacting subjects to make an inference or making a faulty inference in an intersubjective act.
An intersubjective act is interpreted as an inter-action, where communicants' verbal/non-verbal communicative actions are viewed as perceptual stimuli, which trigger parallel conscious/non-conscious inference processes involving cognition, volition and affect resulting in a motivated communicative social action.
Inferential analysis applied in the research provides tools for the recreation of communicants' inferential processes and enables to consider cognitive, perceptual, affective and volitional aspects of interaction stipulating their goal-oriented motivated communicative verbal and non-verbal actions.
American cinema discourse represented by the genre of a situation comedy and modelling live communication supplied instances of communicative failures subjected to analysis.
We claim that differences in structures of communicants' ethological encyclopaedic knowledge are one of the causes of cognitive communicative failures.
Cognitive communicative failures depend on the centrality of the content and structure of encyclopaedic knowledge evoked by verbal/non-verbal communicative actions of interacting subjects in an intersubjective act. Centrality depends on how well a particular conceptual content is established in the communicant's memory as well as on a particular context in which a lingual unit is embedded. We give evidence for ethological knowledge to be of both declarative and procedural nature.
We prove that cognitive communicative failures caused by differences in structures of communicants' ethological encyclopaedic knowledge result from the addresser's ignorance of social values and ethic norms of social behaviour.
Key words: affect, cognition, communicative failure, ethological encyclopaedic knowledge, inference, intersubjectivity, verbal/nonverbal communicative action, volition.
Анотація
Комунікативні невдачі, що зумовлені етологічними чинниками (на матеріалі американського кінодискурсу)
Дубцова О. В. кандидат філологічних наук, доцент кафедри педагогіки, іноземної філології та перекладу Харківський національний економічний університет імені Семена Кузнеця , Харків, Україна
У роботі виявлено й описано комунікативні невдачі, що спричинені розбіжностями у структурах етологічних енциклопедичних знань комунікантів у межах інтерсуб'єктивної парадигми.
Комунікативна невдача розглядається як нездатність суб'єктів інтеракції вивести інференцію або виведення хибної інференції в межах інтерсуб'єктного акту.
Інтерсуб'єктний акт тлумачиться як взаємодія, в межах якої вербальні/невербальні комунікативні дії комунікантів розглядаються як перцептивні стимули, що запускають паралельні свідомі/несвідомі інференційні процеси, які включають когніцію, волевиявлення й афект в ініціацію мотивованої комунікативної соціальної дії.
Інференційний аналіз, застосований у дослідженні, дає інструментарій для відтворення інференційних процесів комунікантів та уможливлює врахування когнітивних, перцептивних, афективних та волевиявних аспектів взаємодії, що зумовлюють їхні цілеспрямовані мотивовані вербальні/ невербальні комунікативні дії.
Випадки комунікативних невдач, що підлягають інференційному аналізу, відібрані з американського кінодискурсу, представленого жанром ситуаційної комедії, який моделює живе спілкування.
Ми стверджуємо, що розбіжності у структурах етологічних енциклопедичних знань комунікантів є однією з причин когнітивних комунікативних невдач.
Когнітивні комунікативні невдачі залежать від центральності змісту та структури енциклопедичних знань, активованих вербальними/невербальними комунікативними діями суб'єктів інтеракції під час інтерсуб'єктного акту.
Центральність залежить від того, наскільки відповідна концептуальна структура є укоріненою/сформованою у свідомості комуніканта, а також від контексту, в якому вживано мовну одиницю.
Ми стверджуємо, що етологічні знання є одночасно декларативними та процедуральними за своєю природою. Ми доводимо, що когнітивні комунікативні невдачі, що спричинені розбіжностями у структурах етологічних енциклопедичних знань комунікантів, є наслідком неусвідомлення адресантом соціальних цінностей та етичних норм соціальної поведінки.
Ключові слова: афект, вербальна/невербальна комунікативна дія, волевиявлення, етологічні енциклопедичні знання, інтерсуб'єктивність, інференція, когніція, комунікативна невдача.
The phenomenon of a communicative failure has captured scientists' attention for many decades. It has been studied from different linguistic perspectives: representatives of formal approaches treat it as a deviation from language norms [15]; some proponents of functional approaches address it as communication disruptions caused by the inability of certain speech patterns to fulfil their functions [8, p. 67], while others stress the role of a non-verbal aspect of communication [12; 20]; within pragmatics it is viewed as the addresser's
failure to achieve perlocutionary goals [2; 5; 28], as the inability to understand “what is meant by what is said” [29, p. 91] or as a result of the divergence between a predicted and actual effect of the utterance [22]; in cognitive studies it is interpreted as the speaker's inability to generate the desired mental state in the mind of his/her communication partner [3] or as an addressee's inability to interpret an utterance, i.e. to correlate an addresser's utterance with his/her own cognitive model in the way expected by an addresser [24].
We claim that all the above-mentioned approaches fail to uncover the roots of a communicative failure. Pragmatic studies of communicative failures are based on Relevance theory [26; 32], which develops H.P. Grice's pragmatic approach to meaning in communication. H.P. Grice [10; 11] assumes that (a) a speaker's meaning is an overtly expressed intention that is fulfilled by being recognized; (b) it has to be inferred from the speaker's behaviour and contextual information; (c) in inferring the hearer is guided by a cooperative principle and conversational maxims. Thus, Relevance theory treats utterance comprehension as “an inferential process which takes as input the production of an utterance by a speaker, together with contextual information, and yields as output an interpretation of the speaker's meaning” [32, p. 3]. Other things being equal, “the greater the cognitive effect achieved, and the smaller the mental effort required, the more relevant this input will be to you at the time” [26, p. 260-266]. This brings us to the point that inference is viewed as a purely rational, logical cognitive procedure.
The notion of inference is also widely employed by cognitive linguists, who refer to it as purely rational cognitive structure, a logical conclusion [9; 19; 27; 31]. We would rather disagree with such an assumption as a number of studies suggest that nonconscious thought processes operate concurrently with the conscious ones: natural thought processes are nonlogical, “arational” [23], underpinned by low-level spontaneous nonlinear connective dynamic where intuition, creativity and insights prove more powerful than linear reasoning [13, p. 216].
Here we side with A.P. Martynyuk defining inference as a “contextually motivated semantic structure, emerging in an intersubjective act as a result of complex parallel conscious and nonconscious multi-level intersubject linkage processes recruiting the multilevel cognitive, volitional and affective elements of the psychic experiential context of the intersubjective act” [21, p. 67].
The notion of intersubjectivity has turned out to be particularly valuable for the analysis of communicative failures. It is viewed as a human capacity of “sharing experiential content (e.g., feelings, perceptions, thoughts, linguistic meanings) among a plurality of subjects” [33, p. 1], “not only, and not primarily, on a cognitive level, but also (and more basically) on the level of affect, perceptual processes and conative (action-oriented) engagements” [33, p. 3].
Thus, going beyond cognitive linguistic theories relying on the embodied model of cognition and focusing on the importance of bodily experience in understanding the nature of linguistic signs [4; 6; 14; 16; 17; 30], the intersubjective model of cognition and communication [21] enables to take a broader look at the process of meaning generation in communication accounting for volition and affect adapting cognitive experience to the needs and feelings of the interacting subjects and triggering their goal-oriented motivated communicative actions [21, p. 65].
The goal of the present paper is to discover and describe cognitive communicative failures caused by differences in structures of communicants' ethological encyclopaedic knowledge.
This goal is achieved through the following objectives:
- to give a definition of a communicative failure from the intersubjective perspective;
- to discover cognitive communicative failures resulting from differences in structures of communicants' ethological encyclopaedic knowledge;
- to describe communicative failures caused by ethological factors within the intersubjectivity paradigm.
To achieve the goal and objectives we employ a unit of analysis called an intersubjective act of communication defined as “an inter-action, structurally including at least two verbal or/and co-verbal utterances: one initial and the other responsive, embedded in the complex dynamic psychic experiential context `shared' by the communicants focusing attention on the same verbal/ co-verbal utterance as a perceptual stimulus which triggers parallel conscious/nonconscious inference processes involving cognition, volition, and affect to issue a command of a meaningful goal-oriented communicative and/or (immediate or postponed) social action” [21, p. 65].
Within the framework of the intersubjectivity paradigm a communicative failure is viewed as an inability of a subject to make any inference or making a faulty inference. Inference is explained both as “the natural emergent product of conscious/nonconscious interplay of volition, cognition, and affect, triggering a motivated communicative and social action” and as “a tool of discovering this key structure of human physic experience in linguistic analysis” [21, p. 69].
Our sample consists of 1000 instances of communicative failures taking place in intersubjective acts extracted from American situation comedy series.
The causes of communicative failures have been identified applying inferential analysis.
Carrying out inferential analysis, a researcher becomes a participant of an intersubjective act assuming the role of an observer interpreting communicative actions of other participants.
While watching TV series, the researcher shares the mental (becomes aware of the events, the participants' relationships, etc.) and physical (has access to all the perceptual stimuli - wording of the utterances, intonation patterns, body language, facial expressions, etc.) context of the intersubjective act.
The task of the researcher-interpreter is to make inferences about the addresser's intended meanings and the addressee's inferences, embodied in their verbal and/or non-verbal communicative actions, and identify causes of communicative failures considering perceptual, cognitive, affective and volitional aspects of interaction triggering their goal-oriented motivated communicative actions.
The results of the inferential analysis suggest that differences in structures of communicants' ethological encyclopaedic knowledge happen to be one of the causes of cognitive communicative failures.
Cognitive communicative failures result from the specificity of the communicants' cognitive experience influencing the content and structure of encyclopaedic knowledge evoked by the verbal/nonverbal communicative action in an intersubjective act. This specificity can be explained in terms of centrality, which depends on 1) how well a particular conceptual content is established (“entrenched”) in the memory; 2) the particular context in which a lingual unit is embedded [18, p. 159].
Encyclopaedic knowledge includes both declarative knowledge (conscious precise memories and recognition of objects and events as expressed through language [1]) and procedural knowledge (implicit memory of psychomotor processes as procedures that have become automatic and non- conscious [1]).
The structural organization of declarative knowledge presupposing conceptual ontology and hierarchy of conceptual structures is covered by R. Langacker's domain theory [18], whereas structuring relations between declarative knowledge of the same hierarchy level is explained within Ch. Fillmore's frame semantics [7]. Schematic arrangement of procedural knowledge about different types of communicative situations presupposing succession of actions is described in terms of a script [25].
Ethological knowledge is associated with both a declarative and procedural content that accumulates experience of social behaviour.
Cognitive communicative failures caused by differences in structures of communicants' ethological encyclopaedic knowledge, as a rule, result from the addresser's ignorance of social values and ethic norms of social behaviour. For example:
Niles and Frasier met at the cafй.
NILES: Alright, the least you could do is say hello to Aunt Patrice.
FRASIER: I'm not driving out to your house.
NILES: You don't have to. She's sitting out in the car.
FRASIER: You left her in the car?!
NILES: I cracked open a window.
FRASIER: Well, then she's fine. (Frasier: season 1, episode 5).
Responding to Niles' rebuke that he should at least greet his aunt, who has just arrived, Frasier says he does not want to go to his brother's house believing that the aunt is there. Frasier's misinterpretation is explained by the fact that it does not occur to him that Niles could leave their aunt in the car while they were sitting in the cafe because it is against ethic norms of social behaviour.
Joey and Michael's apartment - Gina (Joey's sister), Michael (his nephew), and Alex (their neighbour) are in the living room, Joey comes downstairs.
JOEY: Okay, family meeting.
ALEX: Aww, you think of me as _ family?
JOEY: Oh, hey, Alex. I didn't see you there. Um, as you all know, my new show Deep Powder is premiering next week. My official unveiling as a big time celebrity and someone gets to with me to the party as my guest. (Joey: season 1, episode 14).
Joey addresses everyone present calling them “family”. Alex, Joey's neighbour, also includes herself in the reference sphere and creates her question based on REQUESTING A COMPLIMENT - RECEIVING A COMPLIMENT script, as required by etiquette. Instead, Joey insults her with his response based on REQUESTING INFORMATION - PROVIDING INFORMATION script, ignoring ethical standards.
Will's Office. Will is working at his desk as Jack enters with his dog.
WILL: Hey, hey, no muddy paws, and no surprises on the carpet.
JACK: [puppy-talk voice] Klaus Von Puppy is clean and housebroken, thank you very much.
WILL: I wasn't talking about the dog. (Will and Grace: season 1, episode 15).
In the given example the addressee cannot interpret the addresser's utterance correctly as the latter violates ethic norms: Will's remark falls under a WARNING FOR A DOG rather than a WARNING FOR A HUMAN script. However, it turns out that Will addressed Jack but not the dog.
There are cases when it is possible to trace the reason for the addresser's violation of ethic norms:
Joey and Michael's house.
JOEY: Hey, Michael. Who was that girl last night?
MICHAEL: I wasn't with a girl.
JOEY: No, the one I brought home. I can never remember her name. (Joey: season 1, episode 11).
Responding to Joey's question Who was that girl last night? Michael interprets it as a personal question answering that he was not with the girl. In this situation the addressee fails to make the right inference because the addresser's behaviour goes beyond stereotypical expectations: Joey brought the girl home but he does not remember her name and hopes that his neighbour Michael knows it. However, Joey's communicative actions can be explained if we look closer at his personality: he is very popular with women, he sees them a lot, having no intention to develop relationships with anybody, so he does not even bother to remember their names.
Jill is worried as her son broke up with his girlfriend.
JILL: Jennifer's his first love. It hurts when you loose your first love.
TIM: You don't have to worry about that. I see mine every day.
JILL: That's so sweet.
TIM: [walks to the garage] You should meet her sometime! (Home Improvement: season 2, episode 18).
Trying to comfort Jill, who is worried about their son's first love, Tim says he sees his first love every day. Jill interprets this as a COMPLIMENT as she considers herself her husband's first love. However, Tim refers to another woman and creates his verbal utterance based on ARGUMENTATION (PROVIDING FACTS TO PROVE THE OPINION) script. Jill makes a faulty inference presupposing that it is not customary to talk about a past lover in the presence of a current one but it is obvious that her husband does not share this belief and even mentions seeing his first love daily as a mere fact. Thus, we assume that the fact that Tim does not have any feelings for his ex-girlfriend accounts for his communicative actions.
The example given below represents a case of the addresser's disregard for social values prompted by his feelings and attitudes that are inseparable from interests, needs and desires:
FRASIER: I need to talk to you. Here, have a seat, right here. Now listen Niles, I'm having a young lady over on Friday night, I was hoping you could take Dad out for me.
NILES: Oh, I wish you'd said Saturday.
FRASIER: Why, you have plans Friday?
NILES: No, I have plans Saturday. (Frasier: season 1, episode 13).
Frasier asks his brother to spend time with their father on Friday. Niles regrets that Fraser did not ask him to do so on Saturday. Therefore, Frasier interprets his brother's verbal utterance within REFUSAL script. However, Niles actually regrets that he is not busy on Friday and has no reason to turn his brother down.
The addresser's disregard for social values prevents the addressee from making the right inference: the son does not feel sorry for not being able to take care of his father but for being forced to do so having no other plans.
The following dialogue exemplifies a situation in which the addresser's needs and desires prevail over social values:
ZACH: Hey Joey! How'dyou get here before me?
CHUCK: I 'm not Joey. I'm Chuck. I'm his stuntman [goes to shake Zach's hand].
ZACH: [shakes his hand] Hey, I'm Zach. I'm handling craft services. Hey listen, whatever you do, stay away from the roast beef.
CHUCK: Oh why? Is it bad?
ZACH: No, I like it. (Joey: season 2, episode 4).
Chuck interprets Zack's remark about the roast beef as good advice within TAKING CARE OF THE NEIGHBOUR'S INTERESTS script, underpinned by adherence to social values. Alternatively, Zack creates his verbal utterance based on TAKING CARE OF SELF-INTERESTS script, solely driven by his own needs and desires determining his goal-oriented communicative social action.
There are cases when the addressee's psychological state (feelings and emotions), motivating inferencing processes, accompany a communicative failure caused by addresser's ignorance of ethic norms of social behaviour:
MARTIN: Great. How about you and me having a beer together?
FRASIER: Wow. You know, in all these years you've never asked me that. I'd love to have a beer with you, dad.
MARTIN: Well then, you better haul ass, 'cause the store closes in ten minutes. communicative failury ethical american cinematic
FRASIER: Right [exits]. (Frasier: season 1, episode 2).
Interpreting Martin's communicative action as an INVITATION TO DRINK, Frasier makes a faulty inference. This inference can be called emotional as it results from the addressee's psychological state: Frasier was glad that his father, with whom he had a very tense relationship, offered to drink together. However, creating his utterance, Martin relied on REQUESTING TO BUY A DRINK script. It is obvious that the addresser violates stereotypical expectations: usually a person, offering a drink, arranges it.
Phoebe has a twin sister, whose name is Ursula. Joey bought a present for her.
JOEY: Phoebe, could you do me a favour? Could you try this on? I just wanna make sure it fits.
PHOEBE: Oh, my first birthday present. Oh, this is really.
JOEY: Oh, no no no. It's_for Ursula. Ijustfigured, you know, size wise. (Friends: season 1, episode 16).
As it was just before her birthday, Phoebe interprets her friend's request to try on a dress within RECEIVING A GIFT script as it seems to be the most likely interpretation in this situation:
Phoebe was already in the mood to accept presents and thus makes an emotional inference resulting from her psychological state.
However, Joey creates his verbal utterance based on ASKING FOR ADVICE script, which is inappropriate given the ethic norms of social behaviour (it is not a good idea to ask one girl to try on a gift for another girl especially when they have a birthday on the same day). In this situation we would rather suggest that this communicative failure is the result of the `clash' of addresser's cognitive experience (ethological encyclopaedic knowledge being a part of it) and the addressee's psychological state (feelings and emotions) determining their motivated communicative actions.
The necessity of taking into account not only cognitive experience serving as a basis for meaning generation but also volition and affect, adjusting this experience to the communicants' interests, needs, desires and feelings stipulating their goal-oriented motivated communicative verbal and non-verbal actions, derives from understanding communication as an intersubjective phenomenon.
Application of the inferential analysis, having in its foreground the notion of inference as a cognitive operation of acquiring new experience through conscious/nonconscious use of psychic resources including rational thinking, affect and volition, provides new opportunities for the study of the phenomenon of a communicative failure. It offers a new approach to understanding forces driving communicative and social behaviour of the interacting subjects, thus enabling to get insight of communicative failures.
The results of the inferential analysis suggest that cognitive communicative failures stem from the specificity of the communicants' cognitive experience influencing the content and structure of encyclopaedic knowledge evoked by the verbal/nonverbal communicative action in an intersubjective act.
Cognitive communicative failures can be caused by differences in structures of communicants' ethological encyclopaedic knowledge resulting from the addresser's ignorance of social values and ethic norms of social behaviour.
The study opens perspectives for the further inquiry in the specificity of communicative failures in other types of discourse, their further classification and description on the basis of the intersubjective model of communication.
Bibliography
1. Anderson J.R. Language, memory and thought. Hillsdale, NJ : Erlbaum. 1976.
2. Остин Дж.Л. Слово как действие. Новое в зарубежной лингвистике. Москва : Прогресс, 1986. Вып. 17. Теория речевых актов. С. 22-129.
3. Bara B.G. Cognitive pragmatics: the mental processes of communication. Cambridge, MA : MIT Press. 2010.
4. Cienki A. Cognitive Linguistics, gesture studies, and multimodal communication. Cognitive Linguistics. 2016. No. 27(4), pp. 603-618. DOI: 10.1515/cog-2016-0063.
5. Ермакова О.П., Земская Е.А. К построению типологии коммуникативных неудач (на материале естественного русского диалога). Русский язык в его функционировании. Коммуникативно-прагматический аспект. Москва : Наука, 1993. С. 30-64.
6. Fauconnier G. Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge, U.K. : Cambridge University Press. 1997.
7. Fillmore C. Frame semantics. In the Linguistic Society of Korea (Eds.). Linguistics in the morning calm. Seoul : Hanshin. 1982. Pp. 111-137.
8. Городецкий Б.Ю., Кобозева И.М., Сабурова И.М. К типологии коммуникативных неудач. Диалоговое взаимодействие и представление знаний. Новосибирск, 1985. С. 64-78.
9. Graesser A.C., Zwaan R.A. Inference generation and the construction of situation models. / In C.A. Weaver III, S. Mannes and C.R. Fletcher (Eds.). Discourse Comprehension: Essays in Honour of WalterKintsch. Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1995. Pp. 117-139.
10. Grice H.P. Meaning. The Philosophical Review. 1957. No. 66(3), pp. 377-388.
11. Grice H.P. Logic and conversation. / In P. Cole, P., J. Morgan (Eds.). Syntax and semantics, 1975. No. 3. Pp. 41-58. New York : Academic Press.
12. Гудков Д.Б. Теория и практика межкультурной коммуникации. Москва : ИТДГК «Гнозис», 2003. 288 с.
13. Hardy C. Networks of meaning: A bridge between mind and matter. Westport, Connecticut; London : Praeger. 1998.
14. Johnson M. The body in the mind (the bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason). Chicago, L. : Chicago University Press. 1987.
15. Кукушкина О.В. Основные типы речевых неудач в русских письменных текстах: монография. Москва : МГУ, 1998. 285 с. URL: http://www.philol.msu.ru/ ~lex/pdfs/kukushkina_chapter2.pdf.
16. Lakoff G. What is a conceptual system? / In Willis F. Overton, David S. Palermo (Eds.). The Nature and Ontogenesis of Meaning. New Jersey Hove : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1994. Pp. 41-86.
17. Langacker R.W. Discourse in cognitive grammar. Cognitive Linguistics. 2001. No. 12 (2), pp. 143-188. DOI: 10.1515/cogl.12.2.143.
18. Langacker R.W. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Vol. I: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford, CA : Stanford University Press. 1987.
19. Long D.L., Golding J.M., Graesser A.C., Clark L.F. Goal, event, and state inferences: An investigation of inference generation during story comprehension. / In A.C. Graesser and G.H. Bower (Eds.). Psychology of learning and motivation, 1990. Vol. 25. Pp. 89-102. New York : Academic Press. DOI: 10.l016/S0079-7421(08)60250-9.
20. Лосева А.А. К проблеме возникновения речевых конфликтов в билингвальных группах. Учебнометодический комплекс дисциплины «Язык и конфликт» / сост. Вершинина Т.С. Екатеринбург, 2007. С. 215-221.
21. MartynyukA. “Now that the magic is gone” or toward cognitive analysis of verbal/co-verbal communication. Cognition, communication, discourse. 2017. No. 15, pp. 51-72. DOI: 10.26565/2218-2926-2017-15-04.
22. Полякова С.Е. Коммуникативные неудачи в англоязычном политическом дискурсе : автореф. дис. ... канд. филол. наук : спец. 10.02.04 «Германские языки». Санкт-Петербург, 2009. 21 с.
23. Reber A.S. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York : Oxford University Press. 1993.
24. Ringle M.H., Bruce B.C. (1982). Conversation failure. / In W.G. Lehnert and M.H. Ringle (Eds.). Strategies for Natural Language Processing. Hillsdale, New Jersey, London : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1982. Pp.203-221.
25. Schank R.C., Abelson R.P. Script, plans, goals, and understand.: an inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ : Laurence Erlbaum Associates. 1977.
26. Sperber D., Wilson D. Relevance: communication and cognition. Oxford; Cambridge, MA : Blackwell. 1986/1995.
27. Sweetser E. Compositionality and blending: Semantic composition, in a cognitively realistic framework. / In N. Janssen and G. Redeker (Eds.). Cognitive Linguistics: Foundations, Scope, and Methodology. Berlin : Mouton de Gruyter. 1999. Pp. 129-162.
28. Теплякова Е.К. Коммуникативные неудачи при реализации речевых актов побуждения в диалогическом дискурсе (на материале современного немецкого языка) : автореф. дис. на соискание уч. степени канд. филол. наук : спец. 10.02.04 «Германские языки». Тамбов, 1998. 17 с.
29. Thomas J. Cross-cultural pragmatic failure. Applied Linguistics, 1983. No. 4(2), pp. 91-112. DOI: 10.1093/applin/4.2.91.
30. Turner M. Reading minds: the study of English in the age of cognitive science. Princeton, NJ : Princeton University Press. 1991.
31. Wilensky R. Discourse, probability, and inference. / In Roger C. Schank, Ellen Langer (Eds.). Beliefs, Reasoning, and Decision Making. Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1994. Pp. 363-388. DOI: 10.4324/9780203773574.
32. Willson D. Relevance theory. In Y. Huang (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Pragmatics. 2016. Pp. 1-25. Oxford : Oxford University Press. DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199697960.013.25.
33. Zlatev J. Intersubjectivity: What makes us human? / In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, E. Itkonen (Eds.). The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity. 2008. Pp. 1-17. Amsterdam : John Benjamins. DOI: 10.1075/celcr.12.02zla.
References
1. Anderson, J.R. (1976). Language, memory and thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
2. Austin, J.L. (1986). Slovo kak deystvie [How to do things with words]. In B. Yu. Gorodetskiy (Ed.). Novoye v zarubezhnoy lingvistike - New in foreign linguistics, 17, 22-129. Moscow: “Progress” [in Russian].
3. Bara, B.G. (2010). Cognitive pragmatics: the mental processes of communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
4. Cienki, A. (2016). Cognitive Linguistics, gesture studies, and multimodal communication. Cognitive Linguistics, 27(4), 603-618. DOI: 10.1515/cog-2016-0063.
5. Ermakova, O.P., Zemskaya, E.A. (1993). K postroeniyu tipologii kommunikativnykh neudach (na materiale estestvennogo russkogo dialoga) [Toward the typology of communicative failures (based on the natural Russian dialogue)]. In E.A. Zemskaya (Ed.), Russkiy yazyk v ego funktsionirovanii: kommunikativno- pragmaticheskiy aspekt - The Russian language in its functioning: communicative-pragmatic aspect (pp. 30-64). Moscow: Nauka [in Russian].
6. Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
7. Fillmore, C. (1982). Frame semantics. In the Linguistic Society of Korea (Eds.). Linguistics in the morning calm (pp. 111-137). Seoul: Hanshin.
8. Gorodetskiy, B.Yu., Kobozeva, I.M., Saburova, I.G. (1985). K tipologii kommunikativnykh neudach [Toward the typology of communicative failures]. Dialogovoe vzaimodeystvie i predstavlenie znaniy - Dialogical interaction and knowledge presentation (pp. 64-78). Novosibirsk: SO AN SSSR [in Russian].
9. Graesser, A.C., Zwaan, R.A. (1995). Inference generation and the construction of situation models. In C.A. Weaver III, S. Mannes and C.R. Fletcher (Eds.). Discourse Comprehension: Essays in Honour of Walter Kintsch (pp. 117-139). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
10. Grice, H.P. (1957). Meaning. The Philosophical Review, 66(3), 377-388.
11. Grice, H.P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole, P., J. Morgan (Eds.). Syntax and semantics, 3 (pp. 41-58). New York: Academic Press.
12. Gudkov, D.B. (2003). Teoriya ipraktika mezhkulturnoy kommunikatsii [Theory and practice ofintercultural communication]. Moscow: ITDGK “Gnosis” [in Russian].
13. Hardy, C. (1998). Networks of meaning: A bridge between mind and matter. Westport, Connecticut; London: Praeger.
14. Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind (the bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason). Chicago: Chicago University Press.
15. Kukushkina, O.V. (1998). Osnovnye tipy rechevykh neudach v russkikhpismennykh tekstakh [Main types of speech failures in Russian written texts]. Moscow: MGU Retrieved from: http://www.philol.msu. ru/~lex/pdfs/kukushkina_chapter2.pdf [in Russian].
16. Lakoff, G. (1994). What is a conceptual system? In Willis F. Overton, David S. Palermo (Eds.). The Nature and Ontogenesis of Meaning (pp. 41-86). New Jersey Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
17. Langacker, R.W. (2001). Discourse in cognitive grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 12 (2), 143-188. DOI: 10.1515/cogl.12.2.143.
18. Langacker, R.W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Vol. I: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford, CA : Stanford University Press.
19. Long, D.L., Golding, J.M., Graesser, A.C., Clark, L.F. (1990). Goal, event, and state inferences: An investigation of inference generation during story comprehension. In A.C. Graesser and G.H. Bower (Eds.). Psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 25 (pp. 89-102). New York: Academic Press. DOI: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60250-9.
20. Loseva, A.A. (2007). K probleme vozniknoveniya rechevykh konfliktov v bilingvalnykh gruppakh [Toward the problem of verbal conflicts in bilingual groups]. In T.S. Vershina (Ed.), Yazyk i konflikt - Language and conflict (pp. 215-221). Yekaterinburg [in Russian].
21. Martynyuk, A. (2017). “Now that the magic is gone” or toward cognitive analysis of verbal/co-verbal communication. Cognition, communication, discourse, 15, 51-72. DOI: 10.26565/2218-2926-2017-15-04.
22. Polyakova, S.E. (2009). Kommunikativnye neudachi v angloyazychnom politicheskom diskurse [Communicative failures in English political discourse]. Extended abstract of candidate's thesis. Saint Petersburg [in Russian].
23. Reber, A.S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press.
24. Ringle, M.H., Bruce, B.C. (1982). Conversation failure. In W.G. Lehnert and M.H. Ringle (Eds.). Strategies for Natural Language Processing (pp. 203-221). Hillsdale, New Jersey, London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
25. Schank, R.C., Abelson, R.P. (1977). Script, plans, goals, and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.
26. Sperber, D., Wilson, D. (1986/1995). Relevance: communication and cognition. Oxford; Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
27. Sweetser, E. (1999). Compositionality and blending: Semantic composition, in a cognitively realistic framework. In N. Janssen and G. Redeker (Eds.). Cognitive Linguistics: Foundations, Scope, and Methodology (pp. 129-162). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
28. Teplyakova, E.K. (1998). Kommunikativnye neudachi pri realizatsii rechevykh aktov pobuzhdeniya v dialogicheskom diskurse (na materiale sovremennogo nemetskogo yazyka) [Communicative failures in performative speech acts in the dialogue discourse (based on the modern German language)]. Extended abstract of candidate's thesis. Tambov [in Russian].
29. Thomas, J. (1983). Cross-cultural pragmatic failure. Applied Linguistics, 4(2), 91-112. DOI: 10.1093/ applin/4.2.91.
30. Turner, M. (1991). Reading minds: the study of English in the age of cognitive science. Princeton, NJ : Princeton University Press.
31. Wilensky, R. (1994). Discourse, probability, and inference. In Roger C. Schank, Ellen Langer (Eds.). Beliefs, Reasoning, and Decision Making (pp. 363-388). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. DOI: 10.4324/9780203773574.
32. Willson, D. (2016). Relevance theory. In Y. Huang (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Pragmatics (pp. 1-25). Oxford : Oxford University Press. DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199697960.013.25.
33. Zlatev, J. (2008). Intersubjectivity: What makes us human? In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, E. Itkonen (Eds.). The shared mind: perspectives on intersubiectivity (pp. 1-17). Amsterdam : John Beniamins. DOI: 10.1075/celcr.12.02zla.
Размещено на Allbest.ru
...Подобные документы
Description of subject of youth comedy is Eurotrip. Journey of American boy Scotty with friends to the girl-friend Mike to Germany. Acquaintance with soccer fans in London and trip to Paris. Description of culinary dishes the cafe of city Amsterdam.
презентация [1,2 M], добавлен 19.01.2014American artist - Andrew Wyeth was born in 1917 in Chides-ford. By 1945 the personal realistic style of the artist was generated. The basic theme of his jobs was landscapes and village life. The most known and famous product of Wyeth - "Kristin's world".
презентация [1,6 M], добавлен 18.06.2010The development of painting in the USA. The First American Revolution and the young republic. Landscape, history and marine painting. American Museum of Natural History. National Gallery of Art. Leslie Lohman Gay Art Foundation, the Philips Collection.
курсовая работа [74,6 K], добавлен 12.02.2014John Christopher Depp - an American film actor, director, musician, screenwriter and producer. The three-time nominee for "Oscar", winner of the "Golden Globe". Actor filmography and roles, the most successful commercial projects with his participation.
презентация [4,4 M], добавлен 27.01.2014The Brooklyn Bridge is a popular landmark in the New York City. The History and the Structure of the Brooklyn Bridge. The Bridge and American Culture. Ethnic and foreign responses to America, nationalism, memory, commemoration, popular culture.
реферат [13,1 K], добавлен 09.07.2013Pre-cinema inventions. Descriptions of some visual devices which anticipated the appearance of motion-picture camera. The development of cinematography. The problem of genesis of the language of cinema. The ways of organizing theatrical performances.
реферат [17,5 K], добавлен 02.02.2015Аналіз взаємозв’язку між розвитком технологічного процесу та мистецтвом естрадного співу. Специфіка використання технології Live Looping в сучасному естрадному мистецтві та її вплив на розвиток вокальної майстерності та творчих здібностей виконавця.
статья [17,6 K], добавлен 07.02.2018Holiday celebrations in America signify the rich blending of historic traditions from other cultures with the uniquely commemorative nature of the people of the United States. Brief review and description basic national and ethnic holidays of Americans.
курсовая работа [42,3 K], добавлен 02.04.2013Johnny Depp - is an American actor, producer, musician. He has won the Golden Globe Award and Screen Actors Guild award for Best Actor. Depp rose to prominence on the 1980s television series 21 Jump Street, becoming a teen idol. Popular films with Depp.
презентация [1,5 M], добавлен 02.10.2012Short-story biography of American film actress Jennifer Aniston: parents, childhood, friends and beginning of career. The personal life of film star and development of its career is in Hollywood. Rewards of artist, as critic and public confession.
презентация [2,1 M], добавлен 26.10.2013Introduction to business culture. Values and attitudes characteristic of the British. Values and attitudes characteristic of the French and of the German. Japanese business etiquette. Cultural traditions and business communication style of the USA.
методичка [113,9 K], добавлен 24.05.2013Singapore is a cosmopolitan society where people live harmoniously among different races are commonly seen. The pattern of Singapore stems from the inherent cultural diversity of the island. The elements of the cultures of Canada's Aboriginal peoples.
презентация [4,7 M], добавлен 24.05.2012Tarantino is an emblem of postmodernism late XX c. Childhood and youth actor's, career, worldwide popularity. Key stylistic components of Tarantino movies. Quentin Tarantino's movies as a game in the sense of the word, which is understood by children.
эссе [14,7 K], добавлен 11.02.2014Оказание услуг в сфере аттракции для людей со всеми уровнями достатка. Использование улучшенной технологии 3D. Прайс-лист услуг кинотеатра "Sensation". Основные отличия комплекса. Общие затраты на открытие кинотеатра. Анализ рисков и страхование.
бизнес-план [36,9 K], добавлен 19.09.2017Полное имя Мадонны, краткая биографическая справка из жизни. Первый сингл певицы. Песня "Holiday" в десятке лучших синглов в Европе. Мадонна как писательница, выпуск детских книжек. Браки и мужчины певицы. Мадонна как "Material Girl" и "Queen of Pop".
презентация [3,7 M], добавлен 29.01.2012Introduction to the history of the Steinway piano - an uncompromising standard of sound, beauty and investment value. History and development of the company Steinway & Sons. The study of competitiveness, strengths and weaknesses of the marketing company.
презентация [565,2 K], добавлен 04.05.2012The study brief biography and works of the great artist Isaac Levitan. The most famous artwork is gorgeous landscape. A photographic image of the famous master of nature, landscapes of Russian nature, drawings, watercolors and book illustrations.
презентация [789,2 K], добавлен 13.03.2015The study of political discourse. Political discourse: representation and transformation. Syntax, translation, and truth. Modern rhetorical studies. Aspects of a communication science, historical building, the social theory and political science.
лекция [35,9 K], добавлен 18.05.2011American Culture is a massive, variegated topic. The land, people and language. Regional linguistic and cultural diversity. Social Relationships, the Communicative Style and the Language, Social Relationships. Rules for Behavior in Public Places.
реферат [35,1 K], добавлен 03.04.2011Theory of the communicative language teaching. Principles and features of the communicative approach. Methodological aspects of teaching communication. Typology of communicative language activities. Approbation of technology teaching communication.
курсовая работа [608,8 K], добавлен 20.10.2014