A general DP-shell analysis of clausal complements: experimental evidence from the agentivity restriction on Russian cto- and ctoby-dauses

Examining the agency constraint argument for cto- and ctoby-clauses. Consideration of experimental evidence based on the factorial definition of the agency constraint. Consideration and characterization of analysis results for all sentences in Russian.

Рубрика Иностранные языки и языкознание
Вид статья
Язык английский
Дата добавления 19.05.2021
Размер файла 204,1 K

Отправить свою хорошую работу в базу знаний просто. Используйте форму, расположенную ниже

Студенты, аспиранты, молодые ученые, использующие базу знаний в своей учебе и работе, будут вам очень благодарны.

Размещено на http://www.allbest.ru/

St. Petersburg State University

Moscow State University of Education

A general DP-shell analysis of clausal complements: experimental evidence from the agentivity restriction on Russian cto- and ctoby-dauses

Knyazev Mikhail Yu. - Ph.D. in Linguistics; Senior Lecturer of Chair І of Comparative Literary Studies and Linguistics, Higher School of Economics - Saint Petersburg; Assistant of the Department of General Linguistics; Senior Researcher of Institute L of Modern Linguistic Studies

Князев Михаил Юрьевич - Ph.D. (лингвистика); старший преподаватель кафедры сравнительного литературоведения и лингвистики, Национальный исследовательский университет «Высшая школа экономики» в Санкт-Петербурге; ассистент кафедры общего языкознания, Санкт-Петербургский государственный университет; старший научный сотрудник Института современных лингвистических исследований, Московский государственный педагогический университет

Ограничение на агентивность как аргумент в пользу именной функциональной оболочки для сентенциальных актантов: экспериментальное исследование

В статье предлагается единый для всех сентенциальных актантов в русском языке анализ с «DP-оболочкой». Выдвигается гипотеза о том, что в косвенных позициях сентенциальные актанты лицензируются нулевым предлогом. Приводится аргумент, основанный на ограничении на агентивность при придаточных с союзами что и чтобы. Приводятся экспериментальные свидетельства, опирающиеся на факторное определение ограничения на агентивность. На основе экспериментальных данных демонстрируется премущество анализа с нулевым предлогом по отношению к двум другим подходам.

Ключевые слова: сентенциальные актанты, ограничения на одушевленность, нулевой предлог, падеж, экспериментальные методы в лингвистике.

This paper argues for a general DP-shell analysis of clausal complements in Russian. It is proposed that clausal complements are licensed by a null P in Caseless positions. The argument is based on an agentivity restriction on cto- and ctoby-clauses. Experimental evidence is presented that makes use of the factorial definition of the agentivity restriction. Two alternative accounts - in terms of a partial DP-shell and semantic coercion - are discussed. It is shown that the experimental results favor the null P account over the alternatives.

Key words: clausal complements, animacy restriction, null preposition, Case, experimental methods.

Introduction

The commonly-held view that clausal arguments, as opposed to DP arguments, do not need (structural) Case [Pesetsky, 1982; Pesetsky, Torrego, 2011] has been challenged by a number of proposals analyzing clausal arguments as optionally embedded in a (possibly null) DP-shell and thus needing Case at least sometimes [Davies, Dubinsky, 2009; Hartman, 2013; Kastner, 2015]. The evidence for this analysis comes from the fact that in a number of languages, including Russian, clausal arguments, e.g. cto-clauses, can be embedded in an overt DP-shell giving rise to the to, cto-clause construction [Khomitsevich, 2007; Hartman, 2013].

Building on these proposals, Knyazev (2016) advances a more radical claim arguing that (bare) cto-clause complements in Russian are always embedded in a null DP-shell, which is licensed by the “Last Resort” insertion of a null P in oblique/PP positions. The argument is based on the so-called agentivity restriction, namely the fact that cto-clause complements of speech act verbs occupying oblique positions force the agentive reading of the verb even when the verb allows the non-agentive reading with a to, cto-clause.3 The agentivity restriction follows from the interpretation of the null P as the relation hold of holding propositional content, which requires a sentient argument.

This paper considers two alternative explanations of the agentivity restriction that do not utilize the null P. According to the first one, the hold relation arises not by virtue of inserting a null P but as the result of the semantic coercion of the verb that happens whenever a PP/oblique-selecting verb combines with a cto-clause. According to the second one, cto-clauses project a DP-shell only with the non-agentive variants of the verbs while remaining CPs with the agentive variant. Thus the agentivity restriction follows from the violation of the Case requirement with the non-agentive variant without invoking P-insertion.

The aim of this paper is twofold. First, it provides experimental evidence for the agentivity restriction, reporting the results of a formal acceptability judgment study using a 2 * 2 factorial design (see, a.o., [Sprouse et al., 2016]). Second, it discusses the results of an experimental study of a similar agentivity restriction observed with ctoby-clauses [Pekelis, 2014], which provides indirect evidence for the null P account favoring it over the two alternatives discussed above.

The structure of this paper is as follows. In section 2 I introduce the agentivity restriction on cto-clauses and present experimental evidence for it. In section 3 I lay out the null P account and discuss two alternative accounts of the agentivity restriction. In section 4 I turn to the agentivity restriction on ctoby-clauses. I report the results of the experimental study of this restriction and show how these results provide evidence for the null P account. Section 5 concludes.

1. The agentivity restriction

1.1 Introducing the restriction

The argument for the null P account is based on the agentivity restriction, which concerns non-illocutionary uses of speech act verbs, illustrated in (1a)-(1b). Such uses express the natural-meaning relation, where the subject \ 3 The other argument comes from the so-called collocational restriction on c7o-clause

complements of nouns. See [Knyazev, 2016, 2017] for details.

is a natural sign of the state of affairs expressed by the complement [Kissine, 2010]. Interestingly, in such uses cto-clauses are significantly degraded unless they are embedded in an overt DP-shell (realized as the demonstrative to) introduced by the selected P/oblique case; cf. the agentive use in (2).

(1) a. Это

намекает

*(на

то),

что

бар

для

туристов

Eto

namekaet

*(na

to),

cto

bar

dlja

turistov.

this

hints

on

it.ACC

that

bar

for

tourists

`This suggests that the bar is for tourists.'

b. Многое

говорит

*(o

том),

что

здесь

были

поселения.

Mnogoe

govorit

*(o

tom),

cto

zdes'

byli

poselenija.

a lot

says

about

it.LOC

that

here

were

settlements

`Many things indicate that there were settlements here.'

(2) Ученые

намекают /

говорят,

что

здесь

были

Ucenye

namekajut /

govorjat,

cto

zdes'

byli

scientists

say

that

here

were

поселения.

poselenija. settlements

`Scientists say/hint that there were settlements here.'

Crucially, all verbs that show the agentivity restriction disallow the realization of their clausal argument as an accusative DP, as shown in (3), and require an oblique/PP instead, cf. (1).4 By contrast, verbs that do realize their clausal argument as an accusative DP such as dokazyvat' `prove' and podtverzdat' `confirm' do not show the agentivity restriction, as seen in (4).

(3) Что это

*намекает /

77говорит?

Cto eto

*namekaet /

??govorit?

what.Acc this hints

`What does this suggest/indicate?'

says

(4) Это доказывает / подтверждает

{ нашу гипотезу /

Eto dokazyvaet

/ podtverzdaet

{ nasu gipotezu /

this proves

confirms

our hypothesis.Acc

4 Henceforth I will not distinguish between oblique and PP positions assuming, following [Pesetsky, 2013], that positions where oblique-case-marked DPs occur are PP positions by virtue of the fact that oblique cases are uniformly assigned by (possibly null instances of) the category P.

ISSN 2500-2953

Rhema. Рема. 2017. № 4

J

, что

бар

для

туристов

}.

, cto

bar

dlja

turistov

}.

that

bar

for

tourists

`This proves/confirms our hypothesis/ that the bar is for tourists.'

The only exception to this pattern are verbs like ubezdat' `convince' which do not show the agentivity restriction despite realizing their clausal argument as a PP, as shown in (5). Such verbs are special in that they have a causative component to their meaning and an experiencer argument (cf. `convince' ~ `cause to believe').Another example is napominat' `remind', which allows both natural sign and causative reading in its non-agentive uses. Crucially, it shows the agentivity restriction only in its natural sign reading. See [Knyazev, 2016; 2018] for details.

(5) Это

убедило

их

в

том,

что

бар

для

туристов.

Eto

ubedilo

ix

(v

tom),

cto

bar

dlja

turistov.

this

convinced

them

in

it.LOC

that

bar

for

tourists

`This convinced them that the bar is for tourists.'

Thus the agentivity restriction is observed only with cto-clause arguments in oblique/PP positions with verbs that have no agent or experiencer argument.

1.2 Experiment 1

One may note that violations of the agentivity restriction do not yield absolute unacceptability. This leads to the possibility that the proposed restriction is not a real grammatical restriction but rather an epiphenomenon reflecting the perceived `colloquial' flavor of cto-clauses in PP positions, coupled with the overall lower `naturalness' of non-illocutionary uses of speech act verbs. To rule out this possibility, the agentivity restriction was experimentally investigated using a 2 x 2 factorial design with factors clause type and agentivity, as shown in (6) (the design followed studies of island effects such as [Sprouse et al., 2016]). The effect of the agentivity restriction on acceptability was measured by first calculating the sum of the effects of clause type [6a-6b] and agentivity [6a-6c] and then subtracting it from the total effect of the purported violation in (6d) [6a-6d] (obtaining the differences-in-differences, or DD-score). A grammatical effect was operationalized as a positive DD-score reflecting a statistically significant interaction between the two factors. This would be visually represented as an interaction plot with two non-parallel lines with a characteristic `superadditive pattern' (i.e. the joint effect exceeds the sum of the two independent effects).

(6) a. agentive I to, Сто clause russian agency

Иван

намекает

на

то,

что.

Ivan

namekaet

na

to,

cto.

Ivan

hints

on

it.ACC

that

b. AGENTIVE

1 Сто

Иван

намекает,

что...

Ivan

namekaet,

cto...

Ivan

hints

that

C. NON-AGENTIVE | TO, CTO

Это

намекает

на

то,

что.

Eto

namekaet

na

to,

cto.

this

hints

on

it.ACC

that

NON-AGENTIVE | СТО

Это

намекает,

что...

Eto

namekaet,

cto.

this

hints

that

In the experiment (n = 282) six verbs were tested comprised of five verbs showing the agentivity restriction (govorif `say', namekat' `hint', napominat' `remind', ukazyvat' `indicate' and svidetel'stvovat' `testify') and the verb ubezdat' `convince' as the control case.The verb grozit' `threaten', which is also reported to show the agentivity restriction in [Knyazev, 2016], was not included in the experimental sentences because of the clear acceptability contrast. Each verb was tested with two types of non-agentive subjects, i.e. full DP and pronoun, in four conditions as in (6) yielding 6 x 2 x 4 = 48 experimental sentences in sum.See [Knyazev, 2018] for details and materials. The participants were asked to give acceptability judgments on a 1-5 scale. For each of the 6 x 2 = 12 sets a two-way ANOVA (on z-scores) was performed and the DD-scores were obtained. The results are summarized in Table 1 and Figure 1.

As we see in Table 1, the first five verbs showed robust interaction and a DD-score at least as strong as 0.5 (the only exception is namekat' `hint' with a full DP subject, where the effect is weak). Crucially, ubezdat' `convince', which was not predicted to show the agentivity restriction, yielded a different pattern with only a weak effect with both types of subjects.

Table 1 The DD-scores and the p-values for the interaction of the factors clause type and agentivity

говорить govorit' `say'

намекать namekaf `hint'

напоминать napominaf `remind'

указывать ukazyvaf `indicate'

свидетельствовать svidetel'stvovaf `testify'

убеждать ubezdaf `convince'

full NP

DD

0.84

0.34

0.54

0.70

0.99

0.32

P

<0.001

0.02

<0.001

<0.001

<0.001

0.03

pronoun

DD

1.36

0.54

0.74

1.14

1.29

0.29

P

<0.001

<0.001

<0.001

<0.001

<0.001

0.06

Fig. 1 . The mean (normalized) ratings for cto- and to, cto-clauses

To conclude, the results of the experiment largely confirm the existence of the agentivity restriction as an independent grammatical constraint.

2. The account and some alternatives

2.1 The null P account

In order to account for the agentivity restriction, Knyazev (2016) argues that cto-clauses are embedded in a null DP-shell, which is licensed in PP/oblique positions by the `Last Resort' insertion of a null P. The crucial assumption is that the null P is interpreted as the generalized relation hold obtaining between an attitude holder (realized as, whereas agent/experiencer argument of the higher verb), and the proposition (realized as the complement of the null P, henceforth Phold).Krapova (2010) argues for the insertion of null P in clausal complements of factive PPш

verbs in Bulgarian. The difference between Krapova's null P and Phold (see Below) is that О the former is restricted to verbs that take preposition za `for' and complements headed by the complementizer deto found in relative clauses, whereas Phold can substitute for any preposition. Note incidentally that Russian cto in c7o-clauses can also function as a relative f complementizer. It is further assumed that Phold incorporates into V with the semantic effect of predicate conjunction, as in (7).

(7) namekat' `hint' + PHOLD:

Tp s t>.Xx.Xe. hint (e) & Cause/Agent (e,x) & Theme (e,p) & hold (x,p) ф

The agentivity restriction follows from the licensing condition on PHOLD, namely that it requires an attitude holder argument, which is present in the agentive uses and absent in the non-agentive uses, as shown in (8).

(8) Иван /

*это

намекает,

что

он

устал.

Ivan /

*eto

namekaet,

[PP Phold [dp Cto

on

ustal]].

Ivan

this

hints

that

he

tired

X HOLD X

-HOLD

The fact that verbs like dokazyvat' `prove' and podtverzdat' `confirm' do not show the agentivity restriction is expected since these verbs can license accusative Case and thus do not need a null P to license a cto-clause. The lack of the agentivity restriction with verbs like ubezdat' `convince' is also expected as these verbs have an experiencer argument interpreted as an attitude holder.

Despite the empirical adequacy of the null P account of the agentivity restriction, it may raise conceptual objections as it postulates a null element. Thus it would be desirable to explore alternative explanations of the restriction that do not utilize null P.

2.2 A semantic coercion account

The idea that the agentivity restriction stems from the sentience restriction imposed by the relation HOLD can also be implemented by the mechanism of coercion of the verb meaning without postulating a null P.

Suppose that verbs that take clausal arguments cannot by themselves take propositional (<s,t>) arguments and instead uniformly take individual (<e>) arguments. Whenever a verb takes a clausal argument, the argument is nominalized, its type shifted to <e> (see [Potts, 2002]). As a consequence, only verbs that take <e>-type/DP arguments, e.g. dokazyvat' `prove', will be able to take clausal arguments `natively'. By contrast, verbs like namekat' 'hint' that do not take <e>-type/DP arguments, will be unable to take a clausal argument (unless the relation is mediated by overt P, leaving the exact details of the composition aside), as shown in (9a), and will need some special mechanism for this. It is natural to assume that this mechanism is coercion, which augments the meaning of the verb by a relation R introducing a clausal ф argument, as in (9b).9 A good candidate for this argument-introducer is the same HOLD relation that we saw above.

(9) a. namekat' `hint':

Zx.Ze. hint (e) & Cause/Agent (e,x) No argument slot!!

86

9 The idea was suggested to me by Ora Matushansky (p.c.).

(9)b. NAMEKAT' `hint': coercion

7p s t>.Ax.Ae. hint (e) & Cause/Agent (e,x) & Theme (e,y) & hold (x,p)

Whenever verbs like namekat' `hint' take cto-clauses, this leads to coercion and thereby to the agentivity restriction. The proposed account yields the same results as the null P account without postulating any unpronounced structure.

2.3 A partial DP-shell account

The other alternative follows the insight of the null P account that cto-clause complements of the non-agentive variants of speech act verbs are DPs and that the agentivity restriction ultimately stems from a Case requirement violation. However, instead of invoking P-insertion to account for the availability of cto-clauses with the agentive variants, it assumes that clausal arguments in the agentive case are simply CPs and thus do not need Case.

This alternative follows [Kastner, 2015] in assuming that clausal arguments (that-clauses) come in two types depending on whether the verb is presuppositional or non-presuppositional. Presuppositional verbs presuppose the truth or existence of their propositional argument. These are factive verbs like regret, know, remember and the so-called response stance verbs like deny, accept, agree, which express speech acts undertaken with regards to a certain already-existing proposition. Non-presuppositional verbs express the bringing about of the existence of a proposition and include basic propositional attitude verbs like say, think, claim, suspect, etc. Kastner's main claim is that clausal complements of presuppositional verbs are embedded in a (possibly null) DP-shell, whereas complements of non-presuppositional verbs are CPs.

The agentivity restriction directly follows under Kastner's proposal. Given that speech act verbs in their non-agentive uses express the naturalsign relation between two facts [Kissine, 2010], their complements will be factive and thus DPs. Hence they will violate the Case requirement.One problem with this account is that the agentivity restriction is not as strong as is expected from a Case requirement violation. Note that the null P account does not suffer from this problem as the restriction stems from the violation of the licensing condition on the null P, which is semantic in nature. As for the agentive variants, they express volunteered-stance speech acts and thus can be analyzed as CPs.

In order to decide between these three potential accounts of the agentivity restriction, we need to look at a similar restriction observed with ctoby- clauses.

3. Another agentivity restriction

3.1 Pekelis's observation

Pekelis (2014) observes that manipulative verbs in Russian express their complements as either infinitival or subjunctive (ctoby)-clauses, as shown in (10a)-(10b). However, when these verbs are used with non-agentive subjects, ctoby-clauses are degraded, as shown in (10b), cf. (10a). Pekelis lists four verbs pozvojat' `allow', vynuzdat' `force', zastavljat' `force' and trebovat' `require' as showing this restriction to varying degrees.

(10) a. Руководство

/

положение

/

это

позволило

нам

Rukovodstvo

/

polozenie

/

eto

pozvolilo

nam

management

situation

this

allowed

us

увеличить расходы.

uvelicit' rasxody.

increase.iNF expenses

`The management/this allowed us to increase the expenses.'

b. Руководство

/ ??положение

/ *это

позволило, чтобы

мы

Rukovodstvo

/ ??polozenie

/ *eto

pozvolilo, ctoby

my

management

situation

this

allowed that.suBj

we

увеличили

расходы.

uvelicili

expenses.

increased

rasxody

`The management/situation/this allowed us to increase the expenses.'

The restriction in (10b) shares non-trivial properties with the agentivity restriction on cto-clauses discussed in section 2. First, all four verbs listed by Pekelis fail to take accusative DPs in their non-agentive variant, as shown in (11). In contrast, verbs that do take accusative DPs such as predpolagat' `presuppose' allow ctoby-clauses, as shown in (12).11 Second, Pekelis found that the restriction is weaker with non-pronominal subjects, as can be seen in (10b). This parallels the findings reported for cto-clauses in section 2.2.

(11) Что это

Cto eto

what.Acc this.NOM

"^позволяет / -вынуждает / *заставляет / -требует!

??pozvoljaet / ??vynuzdaet / *zastavljaet / ??trebuet?

allow / force / make / require

Intended: `What does this allow/require?'

Other verbs in this class are dopuskat' `permit' andpredusmatrivat' `envisage'.(12) Это

предполагает

{ мгновенное

решение

/

Eto

predpolagaet

{ mgnovennoe

resenie

/

this

, чтобы

, Ctoby that.suBj

presuposes immediate

они сразу приняли

oni srazu prinjali

they at once took

decision.ACC

решение}.

resenie}.

decision

`This presupposes an immediate decision / that they take the decision immediately.'

Given these similarities between the two agentivity restrictions, it is desirable to have a unified account of them.12 But before exploring such an account, I will discuss the results of an experimental study of this restriction.

3.2 Experiment 2

The agentivity restriction on ctoby-clauses was tested using a similar factorial design as in Experiment 1, as shown in (13).13

(13) a. agentive | infin

Иван

позволил

нам

увеличить

расходы.

Ivan

pozvolil

nam

uvelicit'

rasxody.

Ivan

allowed

us

increase.INF

expenses

b. agentive

1 CTOBY

Иван

позволил

нам,

чтобы...

Ivan

pozvolil

nam,

Ctoby...

Ivan

allowed

us

that.suBj

c. NON-AGENTIVE | INFIN

Это

позволило

нам

увеличить

расходы.

Eto

pozvolilo

nam

uvelicit'

rasxody.

this

allowed

us

increase.INF

expenses

d. non-agentive I ctoby

Это

позволило

нам,

чтобы...

Eto

pozvolilo

nam,

ctoby.

this

allowed

us

that.suBj

Table 2 The DD-scores and the p-values for the interaction of the factors clause type and agentivity

позволять pozvoljat' `allow'

вынуждать vynuzdat' `force'

заставлять zastavljat' `make'

требовать trebovat' `force'

full NP

DD

0.61

-0.15

0.03

0.21

p

0.02

not sign.

not sign.

not sign.

pronoun

DD

0.70

0.60

0.20

-0.80

p

0.01

0.01

not sign.

0.01

Fig. 2. The mean (normalized) ratings for infinitival and ctohy-clauses

Now let's see whether each of the three accounts discussed in section 3 is consistent with these results.

3.3 Discussion

Let's start form the partial DP-shell account, which derives the agentivity restriction from a Case requirement violation stemming from the DP- shell analysis of presuppositional complements. Given the semantics of ctoby-clauses in examples like (10a)-(10b), it is unlikely that they are presupposed. Hence this account incorrectly predicts no agentivity restriction for ctoby-clauses.

As for the null P account and the semantic coercion account, both accounts correctly predict the agentivity restriction for verbs pozvol'at' `allow' and vynuzdat' `force'. In the agentive variant the ctoby-clause will be licensed either directly by the verb in the case of pozvol'at' `allow', which licenses the accusative DPs (see (14a)), or via the relation hold' (a minimally different variant of hold for subjunctive complements) in the case of vynuzdat' `force' (see (14b)). In the non-agentive variant, however, the ctoby-clause will lead to a clash between the non-agentive subject and hold' in the case of vynuzdat' `force' (as shown in (14b) for the null P account; the semantic coercion account works similarly). The same is true for the non-agentive pozvoljat' `allow' except that under the null P account PHoLD will be disallowed altogether as the verb does not take PP complements (see also the discussion below).

(14) a. Иван

позволяет мне

{ это /

, чтобы...}

Ivan

pozvoljaet mne

{ eto /

, ctoby...

Ivan

allows

me

this.ACC

that.suBj

b. Иван

/ *Это

вынуждает

меня,

чтобы...

Ivan

/ *Eto

vynuzdaet

menja, [PP Phold

ctoby...]

Ivan

this

forces

me

that.suBj

When it comes to zastavljat' `make', which disallows both DP and PP complements, as shown in (15a), the predictions of the two accounts differ. The null P account correctly predicts no contrast between the agentive and the non-agentive variant since both disallow P-insertion for reasons of c-selection, as shown in (15b). Coercion, however, will fail to be blocked and hence the coercion account will incorrectly predict the agentivity restriction, as shown in (15c).

(15) a. *Иван

заставляет

меня

{

это

/

на

это...}

*Ivan

zastavljaet

menja

{

eto

/

na

eto...}

Ivan

makes

me.ACC

this.ACC

on

this

b. С-selection

*Иван

/ *Это

заставляет

меня,

чтобы...

*Ivan

/ *Eto

zastavljaet

menja,

[PP Phold'

ctoby...|

Ivan

this

makes

me. Acc

that.suBj

c. Иван / *Это ЗАСТАВЛЯЕТ меня,

Ivan / *Eto ZASTAVLJAET menja,

To conclude, the acceptability judgment pattern of zastavljat' `make' favors the null P account over the semantic coercion account. The correlation between a purely syntactic property (the lack of selection for PP) and the lack of the agentivity restriction as defined by the factorial design is expected under the syntactic (null P) account but not under the semantic coercion account.

Conclusion

In this paper I argued - against the standard view - that clausal complements in Russian need Case by virtue of being embedded in a null DP-shell. I also argued that in `Caseless' positions complement clauses are licensed by the “Last Resort” insertion of a null P. The evidence for this view comes from the agentivity restriction on both cto- and ctoby-clauses, which follows from the licensing requirement of the null P. I provided experimental evidence for the agentivity restriction by utilizing a 2 x 2 factorial definition of the agentivity restriction. I discussed two alternative accounts of the restriction - the partial DP-shell account and the semantic coercion account - and showed that only the null P account is fully consistent with the results of the experimentals.

References

1. Davies, Dubinsky, 2009 - Davies W., Dubinsky S. On the existence (and distribution) of sentential subjects. Hypothesis A / hypothesis B: Linguistic explorations in honor of DavidM. Perlmutter. D.B. Gerdts, J.C. Moore, M. Polinsky (eds.). Cambridge, MA, 2009. Pp. 111-128.

2. Hartman, 2013 - Hartman J. Varieties of clausal complementation. Ph.D. Diss. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2013.

3. Kastner, 2015 - Kastner I. Factivity mirrors interpretation: The selectional requirements of presuppositional verbs. Lingua. 2015. Vol. 164. Pp. 156-188.

4. Khomitsevich, 2008 - Khomitsevich O. Dependencies across Phases: From sequence of tenseto restrictions on movement. Ph.D. Diss., Utrecht University, 2008.

5. Kissine, 2010 - Kissine M. Metaphorical projection, subjectification and English speech act verbs. Folia Lingpistica. 2010. Vol. 44. Pp. 339-370.

6. Knyazev, 2016 - Knyazev M. Licensing clausal complements: the case of Cto-claus- z es. Doct. diss. Utrecht University, 2016.

7. Knyazev, 2017 -- Исследование «слабого» грамматического ограничения методами экспериментального синтаксиса: пример придаточных с союзом что в функции сентенциального актанта существительного // Рема. 2017. № 1. С. 22-40. [Knyazev M. Studying a weak grammatical violation with experimental syntax methods: the case of sentential complements of nouns with the complementizer cto. Rhema. 2017. No 1. Pp. 22-40.]

8. Knyazev, 2018 - Князев М. Это говорит, что: Ограничение на сентенциальный актант с союзом что при деагентивных употреблениях глаголов речи // Вопросы языкознания. 2018. В печати. [Knyazev M. A restriction on dausal complements with the complementizer cto with non-agentive uses of speech verbs in Russian. Voprosy Jazykoznanija. 2018. To appear.]

9. Krapova, 2010 - Krapova I. Bulgarian relative and factive clauses with the invariant complementizer deto `that'. Lingua. 2010. Vol. 120. Pp. 1240-1272.

10. Pekelis, 2014 - Пекелис О.Е. Инфинитив vs. придаточное с союзом чтобы: к вопросу о выборе способа оформления сентенциального актанта в русском языке // Вопросы языкознания. 2014. № 4. С. 13-45. [Pekelis О.Е. Infinitiv vs. pridatocnoe s sojuzom ctoby: k voprosu o vybore sentencial'nogo aktanta v russkom jazyke [Infinitive vs. ctoby-clause: Choosing the strategy of sentential argument marking in Russian]. Voprosyjazykoznanija. 2014. No. 4. Pp. 13-45.]

11. Pesetsky, 1982 - Pesetsky D. Paths and categories. Doct. Diss. MIT, Cambridge, MA, 1982.

12. Pesetsky, Torrego, 2011 - Pesetsky D., Torrego E. Case. Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Minimalism. C. Boeckx (ed.). Oxford, 2011.

13. Potts, 2002 - Potts C. The Syntax and Semantics of as-parentheticals. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory. Vol. 20. Pp. 623-689.

14. Sprouse et al., 2016 - Experimental syntax and the cross-linguistic variation of island effects in English and Italian. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory. 2016. Vol. 34. Pp. 37-44.

Размещено на Allbest.ru

...

Подобные документы

  • The process of scientific investigation. Contrastive Analysis. Statistical Methods of Analysis. Immediate Constituents Analysis. Distributional Analysis and Co-occurrence. Transformational Analysis. Method of Semantic Differential. Contextual Analysis.

    реферат [26,5 K], добавлен 31.07.2008

  • Systematic framework for external analysis. Audience, medium and place of communication. The relevance of the dimension of time and text function. General considerations on the concept of style. Intratextual factors in translation text analysis.

    курс лекций [71,2 K], добавлен 23.07.2009

  • General characteristics of the stylistic features of English articles, the main features. Analysis of problems the article in English as one of the most difficult. Meet the applications of the definite article, consideration of the main examples.

    доклад [15,8 K], добавлен 28.04.2013

  • Moscow is the capital of Russia, is a cultural center. There are the things that symbolize Russia. Russian’s clothes. The Russian character. Russia - huge ethnic and social mixture. The Russian museum in St. Petersburg. The collection of Russian art.

    реферат [12,0 K], добавлен 06.10.2008

  • General characteristics of the gerund. Predicative constructions with the gerund. The use of the gerund and the function of the gerund in the sentence. The gerund and the other verbals. Comparison of the English gerund and its equivalents in Russian.

    курсовая работа [50,5 K], добавлен 07.11.2010

  • Loan-words of English origin in Russian Language. Original Russian vocabulary. Borrowings in Russian language, assimilation of new words, stresses in loan-words. Loan words in English language. Periods of Russian words penetration into English language.

    курсовая работа [55,4 K], добавлен 16.04.2011

  • The history of football. Specific features of English football lexis and its influence on Russian: the peculiarities of Russian loan-words. The origin of the Russian football positions’ names. The formation of the English football clubs’ nicknames.

    курсовая работа [31,8 K], добавлен 18.12.2011

  • A conservative-protective or right-monarchist as one of the most influential trends in Russia's socio-political movement of the early XX century. "Russian assembly", "Russian Monarchist Party, the Union of Russian people" and "Union of Russian People".

    реферат [12,0 K], добавлен 14.10.2009

  • The literary and art bohemia sharply opposing to weight, singularity and sharpness of experiences. The magic, spiritism and theosophy for works of art. The statement on a boundary of centuries. The role in the "Silver age" of Russian symbolists.

    реферат [16,3 K], добавлен 24.11.2010

  • Definition and classification of English sentences, their variety and comparative characteristics, structure and component parts. Features subordination to them. Types of subordinate clauses, a sign of submission to them, their distinctive features.

    курсовая работа [42,6 K], добавлен 06.12.2015

  • History of the Foreign Intelligence. Structure of the U.S. intelligence community. Legislation on intelligence. Essence of soldiery and state secrets. The intelligence organizations of the Ministry of Defense. within the U.S. civilian agencies.

    реферат [20,5 K], добавлен 23.06.2010

  • Analysis and description of polynational options of English. Different the concepts "version" and "option" of English. Studying of the main problems of loans of a foreign-language element. consideration of a territorial variation of English in Australia.

    курсовая работа [52,5 K], добавлен 08.04.2016

  • The functions of proverbs and sayings. English proverbs and sayings that have been translated into the Russian language the same way, when the option is fully consistent with the English to Russian. Most popular proverbs with animals and other animals.

    презентация [3,5 M], добавлен 07.05.2015

  • Proverbs and sayings are popular genre of English culture. Translation of sayings and proverbs about Work, Love and Wearing from English into Russian. Definition of proverbs and saying. Difference between proverbs and saying. Methods of their translating.

    курсовая работа [49,1 K], добавлен 27.04.2013

  • Theoretical evidence and discuss on idiomatic English: different definitions, meaning, structure and categories of idioms. Characteristic of common names. Comparative analysis and classification of idiomatic expressions with personal and place names.

    курсовая работа [151,4 K], добавлен 11.01.2011

  • Defining cognitive linguistics. The main descriptive devices of frame analysis are the notions of frame and perspective. Frame is an assemblage of the knowledge we have about a certain situation, e.g., buying and selling. Application of frame analysis.

    реферат [324,4 K], добавлен 07.04.2012

  • Definitiоn and features, linguistic peculiarities оf wоrd-fоrmatiоn. Types оf wоrd-fоrmatiоn: prоductive and secоndary ways. Analysis оf the bооk "Bridget Jоnes’ Diary" by Helen Fielding оn the subject оf wоrd-fоrmatiоn, results оf the analysis.

    курсовая работа [106,8 K], добавлен 17.03.2014

  • Research of the main representatives of prose XX of century. Consideration of similarity and distinction genres of leading writers Conrad and Somerset. The analysis of products "Human bondage" and "Human heart" as symbols of a wave of human development.

    курсовая работа [74,8 K], добавлен 08.04.2010

  • The history and legal significance of "de facto marriage" in Russia. The study of value-family relations in the cell of society. Consideration of the sociological methods of investigation of the phenomenon of civil marriage in the Russian society.

    реферат [24,4 K], добавлен 13.09.2010

  • English idioms and their Russian equivalents. Semantic, Stylistic Identity of Translating. The Difficulties of Translation. Pedagogical implications idiomatic tasks in classes. Phraseological fusions, phraseological unities, phraseological collocations.

    презентация [911,6 K], добавлен 03.01.2013

Работы в архивах красиво оформлены согласно требованиям ВУЗов и содержат рисунки, диаграммы, формулы и т.д.
PPT, PPTX и PDF-файлы представлены только в архивах.
Рекомендуем скачать работу.