Vowel production in aphasia: preliminary acoustic findings from arabic

The temporal and spectral characteristics of vowels produced by Palestinian Arabicspeaking individuals with Broca’s aphasia compared to healthy speakers. Vowel duration and formant frequency measures. Detection of overlap of phonetic categories.

Рубрика Иностранные языки и языкознание
Вид статья
Язык английский
Дата добавления 10.04.2023
Размер файла 484,0 K

Отправить свою хорошую работу в базу знаний просто. Используйте форму, расположенную ниже

Студенты, аспиранты, молодые ученые, использующие базу знаний в своей учебе и работе, будут вам очень благодарны.

Размещено на http://www.allbest.ru/

The American University of the Middle East

VOWEL PRODUCTION IN APHASIA: PRELIMINARY ACOUSTIC FINDINGS FROM ARABIC

Adam Hisham

Kuwait

Abstract

vowel aphasia phonetic arabicspeaking

The present study examined the temporal and spectral characteristics of vowels produced by Palestinian Arabicspeaking individuals with Broca's aphasia compared to healthy speakers. Specifically, vowel duration and formant frequency measures (F1&F2) were made. Eight speakers with Broca's aphasia and eight healthy speakers participated in the study. Repetition was used to elicit the target vowels to minimize language comprehension or reading difficulties by the individuals with Broca's aphasia. The speech sample contains the long vowels (/a:,u:,i:/) embedded in CVC words with nasal initials. Statistical analysis was used to determine any significant differences concerning the formant values (F1/F2) between the speaker groups. Findings showed that speakers with Broca's aphasia produced vowel categories with more variability than healthy speakers. For some, formant frequencies were comparable with those of the healthy speakers, and yet others showed an overlap of phonetic categories. Specifically, F1 and F2 for the vowel /u:/ were higher for individuals with Broca's aphasia compared to normal-speaking individuals, but they were not statistically significant. Furthermore, although speakers with Broca's aphasia produced longer vowel durations than the healthy speakers, they were not significantly different between speaker groups except for the vowel /i:/. Reduced vowel space was also observed in speakers with Broca's aphasia. The vowel ellipses as shown by speakers with Broca's aphasia were larger than those of healthy speakers. Results support previous studies showing abnormal temporal durations and spectral patterns in Broca's aphasia. Findings can contribute to cross-linguistic research on vowel production in aphasia, mainly in understudied languages such as Arabic.

Keywords: Palestinian Arabic-speaking Broca's aphasics, formants of Arabic vowels, vowel duration, acoustic analysis, Broca's aphasia.

Анотація

Гішам Адам. Породження голосних під час афазії: попередні результати досліджень звуків у носіїв арабської мови.

У цьому дослідженні порівнювалися темпоральні й спектральні характеристики голосних, що породжувалися носіями палестинської арабської з афазією Брока, та здоровими носіями цієї мови. Зокрема, було зроблено вимірювання тривалості голосних і формантної частоти (F1&F2). У дослідженні взяли участь вісім осіб з афазією Брока та вісім здорових осіб. Повторення використовувалося для виявлення цільових голосних, щоб звести до мінімуму труднощі розуміння мови або читанням особами з афазією Брока. Фрагмент мовлення містив довгі голосні (/a:,u:,i:/) у словах, побудованих за принципом приглосний-голосний-приголосний із початковим носовим звуком. Для визначення будь-яких істотних відмінностей щодо значень формант (F1/F2) між групами мовців використовувався статистичний аналіз. Результати засвідчили, що мовці з афазією Брока породжували голосні з більшою варіативністю, ніж здорові мовці. Для деяких формантні частоти були відповідними з частотами здорових мовців, а в інших виявилося взаємонакладення фонетичних категорій. Зокрема, F1 і F2 для голосних /u:/ були вищими, але статистично не значущими в осіб із афазією Брока в порівнянні зі здоровими мовцями. Більше того, хоча у мовців з афазією Брока була довша тривалість голосних, ніж у здорових мовців, вони не відрізнялися суттєво між групами мовців, за винятком голосного /і:/. Зменшений простір між голосними також спостережено в осіб з афазією. Еліпси голосних, у них були більшими, ніж у здорових мовців. Результати підтверджують попередні дослідження, які показують аномальну часову тривалість і спектральні моделі афазії Брока. Одержані висновки можуть сприяти міжлінгвістичним дослідженням щодо породження голосних під час афазії, головним чином у маловивчених мовах, зокрема арабській.

Ключові слова: носії палестинської арабської з афазією Брока, форманти арабських голосних, тривалість голосних, голосовий аналіз, афазія Брока.

Introduction

Aphasia is a language disorder affecting all language modalities, such as using, speaking, understanding, reading, and writing. Aphasia can be categorized into different types and sub-types, depending on symptom or site of lesion (Garraffa & Fyndanis, 2020).). Table 1. presents the main types of aphasia according to neoclassical and functional system theories. Broca's aphasic speech is widely investigated compared to other types of aphasia (Matchin, 2018).

Substitutions, omissions, deletions, and morphosyntactic impairments are mainly characterized by agrammatic speech (Menn & Obler, 1990; Caramazza, 1997; Haley, Ohde & Wertz, 2011; Boye & Bastiaanse, 2018; Ouden et al., 2019; Martinez - Ferreiro et al, 2020). Deficits in the articulatory implementation of the phonetic features were also observed in speakers with Broca's aphasia (Kurowski & Blumstein, 2016; Verhaegen et al., 2019) reflecting timing deficits and articulatory coordination and laryngeal motor control impairments.

Table 1

Classification of Aphasia According to Neo-Classical (Boston) and Functional System (Luria) Theories Adapted from Johansson (2012, p. 15)

Dichotomisation

Functional systems

Neo-classical (Boston) (Luria)

Anterior / non-fluent /

Broca's aphasia

Efferent motor aphasia

expressive

Transcortical motor aphasia Dynamic aphasia

Global aphasia

Conduction

aphasia

Posterior / fluent /

(subcortical

Afferent motor aphasia

Receptive

Acoustic-gnostic

Wernicke's aphasia

(Sensory) aphasia

Transcortical

sensory

aphasia

Acoustic-mnestic aphasia

Semantic (amnestic)

Anomic aphasia

aphasia

Arabic vowel system

Modern Standard Arabic (MSA) vowel system has three short vowels (/a, i, u,/) and three long counterparts (/a:, i:, u:/). Vowel duration in Arabic is phonemic. That is, vowel quantity results in meaning differences such as in the following minimal pair: [mal] bored [ma:l] moved. English vowel system has been extensively studied compared to Arabic. Several acoustic studies on English vowels have established normative data for formant frequencies. Formant frequencies of standard Arabic vowels, in contrast, were generally less studied and documented.

A very few acoustic studies on Arabic vowels in standard Arabic and Arabic dialects have been carried out (Belkaid 1984; Abou Haidar 1994; Al-Anani, 1999; Newman & Verhoeven, 2002; Alotaibi & Hussain, 2009). It must be noted that these acoustic studies generally did not establish a database for standardized formant frequencies of the Arabic vowels due to research methodologies differences and dialectal and regional linguistic features. Furthermore, these studies were not aiming to study Arabic vowels' acoustic characteristics in individuals with impaired speech and for clinical purposes generally. The current study is thus a step towards filling this gap.

The purpose of the present study was to examine acoustic characteristics of vowels in Palestinian Arabic-speaking individuals with Broca's aphasia compared to healthy speakers. This study has two main research questions:

1. What are the temporal features of vowels produced by speakers with Broca's aphasia compared to healthy speakers?

2. What are the spectral characteristics (F1&F2) of vowels produced by speakers with Broca's aphasia compared to healthy speakers?

We hypothesized that some, if not all, of the acoustic measures of vowel duration and formant frequencies (F1& F2) will be significantly different between the speaker groups.

Temporal and spectral studies

Several studies have addressed the consonantal errors in speakers with Broca's aphasia (Menn & Obler, 1990; Fyndanis et al., 2018; Aziz et al., 2020), assuming that the vowel system is relatively intact in Broca's aphasia. However, the literature on vowel production in aphasia has reported several findings. Haley, Ohde, and Wertz (2001) documented a high frequency of vowel errors in speakers with apraxia of speech with coexisting aphasia. The authors reported distortion and substitution vowel errors.

Keller (1978) examined in a detailed study vowel production and substitution patterns in the speech of five English-speaking Broca's aphasics. The analysis revealed that Broca's aphasics selectively impaired vowel height, and the substituted vowels closely matched the target vowels with the tendency of tongue body lowering. In investigating vowel durations in speakers with Broca's aphasia, Schirmer (2004) found that those durations were significantly longer for speakers with Broca's aphasia than healthy speakers. In contrast, Gandour and Dardarananda (1984) found that Broca's aphasics' durational patterns are comparable to those of the healthy speakers.

Some acoustic studies showed that vowel formant frequency and the related vowel space measures are crucial to vowel discrimination tasks for healthy speakers and individuals with impaired speech (Liu et al, 2005; Tsao et. al., 2006) as they provide reliable acoustic clues about vowels accuracy production (Chodroff & Wilson, 2020). The vowel space is traditionally plotted as the triangular organization showing the first two formants F1 and F2. The acoustic investigations use the vowel space to quantify the articulatory configuration and function (Ferguson & KewleyPort, 2007).

Haley, Bays and Ohde (2001) examined the acoustic and perceptual correlates of vowel quality in speakers with aphasia and apraxia of speech (AOS). Twenty speakers with and without apraxia of speech and ten healthy speakers participated in the study. F1 and F2 were measured at the midpoint of the vowel segment. Their acoustic analysis showed that the formant patterns of speakers with Broca's aphasia were deviated from those of the healthy speakers and varied across the aphasic and apraxic speakers. The results also showed that F1 and F2 values were abnormally variable across the groups. The authors generally documented vowel articulation impairments in many aphasic and apraxic participants, reflecting articulatory positioning deficits for vowel production.

In a repetition task, Ziegler and Hoole (1989) reported F1 and F2 measures in an acoustic and perceptual analysis for the vowels [i] and [i:]. Two aphasic patients, one has Broca's aphasia, and the second has Wernicke's aphasia, together with a healthy speaker and a dysarthric control, participated in the study. The aphasic group demonstrated a higher degree of spectral overlap between the target vowels compared to the healthy speakers. Additionally, the formant frequencies for the vowels produced by the speaker with Broca's aphasia were distributed relatively in a greater range compared to the speaker with Wernicke's aphasia.

Method

Participants

Eight Arabic-Palestinian speakers diagnosed with Broca's aphasia participated in the present study (6 male speakers and two female speakers). They were native speakers of Palestinian Arabic and all diagnosed with mild to moderate Broca's aphasia (Paradis & Libben, 1987). Each participant underwent a standardized aphasia assessment based on both formal and informal tests and clinical reports. The selected subjects were right-handed and had been suffering from a single left hemisphere lesion for at least six months before testing. They showed typical effortful, nonfluent, telegraphic speech features, producing simplified and short utterances. They also had problems with free and bound grammatical morphemes.

The age of the participants ranged from 47 to 70 years. The time post-onset ranged from 13 to 210 months, and their number of years of education ranged from 5 to 14 years. Hearing was normal with no evidence of dysarthria or visual impairments. Table 2 presents the details of Broca's participants. Eight healthy speakers of Palestinian Arabic matching the speakers with Broca's aphasia for age and gender served as the control group.

Table 2

Data of the Participants with Agrammatism Education Handedness m.p.o Handedness Subject Gender Age (years) Hemiplegia

A1

M

69

12

Right

112

Right

Right

A2

M

76

9

Right

30

Right

Right

A3

M

48

13

Right

42

Right

Right

A4

M

47

8

Right

34

Right

Right

A5

M

53

14

Right

120

Right

Right

A6

M

66

10

Right

13

Right

Right

A7

F

69

11

Right

210

Right

Right

A8

F

70

5

Right

70

Right

Right

Means

60.25

10.25

78.87

Participant data: (A: agrammatic speaker; CVA: Cerebro vascular accident; L: left hemisphere; MPO: months post onset; M: male, Edu: Education).

Speech sample

The speech sample includes the target long vowels (/a:/, /u:/, /i:/) embedded in CV disyllabic words with nasal initials (/m, n/). Acoustically, vowels can be easily determined by using nasals, and the coarticulatory effects can be minimized. The stimuli include real words whenever possible; otherwise, nonsense words matching the same structure of the given real words were provided, as shown in table 2. For the target vowel initiation, a carrier sentence has been used. The stimuli have been written on a card and presented to the participants.

Each participant was asked to repeat after the examiner the token 3 times at normal pitch and loudness as much as possible. The aphasic subjects have produced 144 vowel tokens (6 words x 3 repetitions x 8 subjects). Methodologically, the study uses repetition to elicit the target vowels to minimize possible language comprehension, and reading difficulties may occur by the aphasic speakers (Kempler & Van Lancker, 2002).

The recordings were done in a quiet room using a high-quality microphone positioned around 15 centimeters from the participants' mouth. PRAAT program was used to extract the formant frequencies (F1, F2) automatically (Boersma & Weenink, 2020) and manually corrected at the midpoint of the vowels, using Phono-Lab (Metoui, 1995). Formants were measured 10 times in each vowel, and the data were recorded using a sampling rate of 16 Hz at 16 bits. The formants' values of the healthy speakers served as a point of reference. ANOVA statistical analysis was conducted to determine possible significant differences in the formant values (F1/F2) between the speaker groups. The correlation between the vowel formants F1 and F2 was tested using independent sample t-tests.

Table 3

The Stimuli in a Nasal Conlexl

Target Vowels

Target consonant /m/

Target consonant /n/

/a:/

/ma:l/

/na:l/

/u:/

/mu:l/

/nu:l/

/i:/

/mi:l/

/ni:l/

Results

Formant frequencies. Tables 4 and 5 present the mean F1 and F2 values of the long vowels (/a:, u:, i:/) across the nasal consonants /m/ and /n/ and between the speaker groups (Speakers with Broca's aphasia vs. Healthy speakers). Mann-Whitney U-test was used to compare F1 and F2 values across and within the groups (Speakers with Broca's aphasia vs. Healthy speakers).

Table 4

Mean F1 and F2 (Hz) Values of Both the Groups Across Different Nasal Consonants /m/ and /n/V owels /m/ /n/

Group I

Group 2

Group I

Group 2

(Healthy

(Broca's

(Healthy

(Broca's

speakers)

aphasics)

speakers)

aphasics)

F1 F2

F1 F2

F1 F2

F1 F2

/a:/

760.70

1212.31

838.07

1271.75

762.72

1339.97

819.51

1318.72

/u:/

366.75

823.94

432.12

930.66

365.33

1047.53

391.79

1048.71

/i:/

414.72

2584.37

415.18

2126.97

321.59

2256.28

345.70

2334.37

Table 5

Mean F1 and F2 (Hz) of Vowels Between the Speaker Groups

Vowels

Group 1 (Healthy speakers)

Group 2

(Broca's aphasics)

F1 F2

F1 F2

/a:/

761.71 1276.14

828.79 1295.235

/u:/

366.04 935.735

411.955 989.685

/i:/

368.155 2420.325

380.44 2230.67

The speaker groups displayed a statistically significant distinction between vowel targets. Tests of fixed effects showed that F1, (F(1.14)= 8.52, p=.001), but not F2, (F(1.14) = 1.12, p=.33) was significantly different for (/a:,u:, i:/) targets among healthy speakers. The same result was observed in the Broca's aphasic group, where a significant difference between /a:/, /u:/ and /i:/ was found for F1, (F(1.14)=5.31, p=.004), but not F2, (F(1.14)= 1.07, p=.38).

As shown in table 6, the Mann-Whitney U-test results revealed that the F1 values of vowel /a:/ in individuals with Broca's aphasia were statistically significant (Z=-3.267, p=.001) than normal speaking subjects. In contrast, there was no significant difference between speakers with Broca's aphasics and normal speaking individuals for F2. i.e., F2 values were similar between both the groups. For the vowel /u:/, both F1 and F2 were higher in individuals with Broca's aphasia than normal-speaking individuals. But these findings were not statistically significant. The F1 of vowel /i:/ was similar between Broca's aphasics and normal speaking subjects, i.e., no significant difference between the groups on F1 values. However, for F2, it was higher in normal speaking subjects than in individuals with Broca's aphasia, which was statistically significant at.05 level (Z=-3.126, p=.012).

Table 6

The Values of Mann-Whitney U-test for the Vowels /a:, u:,i:) Following the Nasals /m/ and /n/

Vowels

Group I (Normal)

Group 2 (Broca's

aphasics)

F1

F2

F1

F2

/a:/

-2.894

-.714

-3.267

-.672

(p=.037)

(p=.627)

(p=.001)

(p=.664)

/u:/

-1.156

-1.548

-2.351

-2.232

(p=.139)

(p=.097)

(p=.056)

(p=.054)

/i:/

-1.956

-3.126

-1.864

-2.945

(p=.063)

(p=.012)

(p=078)

(p=.023)

* Significant difference between means at P <.05 or less utilizing the MannWhitney U-test (z -store correction for tied ranks).

Vowel duration. Figure 1 presents the average durations (ms) of the three long vowels (/a:,u:,i:/) for speakers with Broca's aphasia and healthy speakers. Although Broca's aphasics produced longer durations for all three vowels than the healthy speakers, the ANOVA analysis showed no significant differences among the speaker groups for the vowels /a:/ and /u:/ except for the vowel /i:/ being significantly longer for speakers with Broca's aphasia relative to the healthy speakers.

Figure 1 Box Plot of Mean Vowel Duration (Ms) for the Vowels /A:, U:,I:/ As Produced by the Healthy Speakers and Speakers with Broca's Aphasia

Vowel space. Figure 2 illustrates the acoustic vowel space of Broca's aphasics and healthy speakers. When comparing Boca's aphasics' finings to healthy speakers, differences between the groups were observed. More specifically, the front vowel /i:/ is more backed in Broca's aphasics than healthy speakers. Vowel backness is associated with F2 lowering, indicating reduced tongue advancement. Interestingly, Boca's aphasics produced /u:/ more fronted compared to the healthy speakers. Also, the acoustic vowel space showed that Broca's aphasics had the vowel categories approximately in the same regions of the vowel space in healthy speakers. Furthermore, unlike the healthy speakers, /a:/ was lower in the acoustic space in Boca's aphasics speech. Furthermore, speakers with Boca's aphasia produced (/i:, u:/) lower in the acoustic space than the healthy speakers whose (/i:, u:/) were located much higher.

As shown in Fig. 2, Broca's aphasics demonstrated a relatively reduced acoustic area compared to healthy speakers. The vowel ellipses of all vowel understudies in Broca's aphasics were noticeably larger than the healthy speakers, as can be seen in Fig. 3. Among the three vowels, Broca's aphasics produced the vowel /u:/ with the largest ellipse areas relative to the healthy speakers, suggesting tongue positioning deviations.

Figure 2 The Acoustic Vowel Space of Broca's Aphasics and the Healthy Speakers

Figure 3 The Acoustic Vowel Space of Broca's Aphasics and the Healthy Speakers

Furthermore, individual formant spaces showed notable variations regarding vowel placement patterns with most vowel distribution variability and overlap for all speakers with Broca's aphasia except for th e participant APH6, as shown in Fig. 4. In contrast, the healthy speakers showed no overlap while producing the target vowel categories, as presented in Fig. 4. The findings generally revealed differences between Broca's aphasics and the healthy speakers concerning vowel height and vowel frontness/backness.

Figure 4 F1 and F2 Frequencies for Normal Speakers (NOR)

Colored diamonds and circles represent the target vowels. There is no distribution overlap for healthy speakers NOR1 and NOR2.

Figure 5 F1 and F2 Frequencies for Speakers Diagnosed with Aphasia (APH)

Colored diamonds and circles represent the target vowels. There is a distribution overlap for the speakers with Broca's aphasia APH1, APH4 and APH7 with the exception of APH6.

Discussion

The present study was aimed to examine vowel production in Arabic-speaking individuals with Broca's aphasics compared to healthy speakers, focusing on vowel duration and formant frequencies. The findings showed that speakers with Broca's aphasia had longer vowel durations compared to healthy speakers, but no statistically significant differences were observed, except /i:/ being longer by speakers with Broca's aphasia. These results are consistent with previous studies (Baum & Pell, 1997; Baum & Boyczuk, 1999; Schirmer, 2004; Seddoh, 2008).

Such excessive prolongations indicate temporal impairments and motor planning deficits. They may also suggest inadequate tactile feedback during vowel production compared to consonants. Therefore, this may make Broca's aphasics are uncertain about the articulatory configurations and tongue movements required for producing vowels accurately and intelligibly (Williams & Seaver, 1986). The fact that slow speech also characterized Broca's aphasia supports such abnormal temporal durations, indicating self-monitoring problems and motor speech planning deficits. It is also interesting to note that our speakers with Broca's aphasia successfully followed the same durational pattern demonstrated by the control group, namely by producing the high vowels with shorter durations than the low vowels. Similar findings were also reported by Gandour and Dardarananda (1984).

The results also showed that speakers with Broca's aphasia had significantly higher F1 values than healthy speakers. The present study results are consistent with previous studies reporting deviated formant patterns in speakers with Broca's aphasics compared to healthy speakers (Keller, 1975; Ziegler & Hoole, 1989; Haley at al., 2001). Additionally, our findings generally support the common consensus that formant frequencies can predict severity and variability among speakers with Broca's aphasia (Ryalls, 1986; Liu et al, 2005; Tsao et al., 2006).

Some studies have found a positive correlation between F1 values and gender, with F1 values being higher in female speakers than male speakers (Kim et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2016). However, gender distribution between our Broca's aphasics was not equal, where only two females participated in the study (2/8 females, see table 2). So, it is unlikely that this asymmetric gender distribution drives high F1 values. Therefore, although we view this gender imbalance as a limitation of the current study, there is no substantial evidence that this may have affected the results introduced here. However, higher F1 values generally suggest an articulatory setting with more oral cavity opening and lowered mandible (Lee et al., 2016).

The vowel acoustic spaces and ellipses areas (Figures 2 and 3) showed that speakers with Broca's aphasia displayed more dispersions in the back vowels than the front vowels (Figures 2 and 3). As mentioned earlier, the acoustic features of vowel formant frequencies indicate tongue placement. The results suggest that speakers with Broca's aphasia showed imprecise articulatory positioning of tongue height and advancement, indicating vowel fronting and backing.

The inspection of the acoustic vowel space in our speakers with Broca's aphasics showed a reduced vowel acoustic area and more centralized vowels than the healthy speakers, indicating tongue positioning constraints, confirming previous findings (Ryalls, 1986). The acoustic analysis also suggests that Broca's aphasics in the present study are less intelligible due to their relatively rescued size of vowel space as compared to the healthy speakers. Such results provide strong evidence to previous findings indicating that the acoustic vowel space can serve as an index for vowel accuracy and speech intelligibility (Kim et al., 2011; Karlsson & van Doorn, 2012).

Speakers with aphasia in the present study also reported variability of formant frequencies. The results also revealed acoustic overlaps between the vowels categories in Broca's aphasics, as shown in figure 5. These findings are consistent with those reported by Haley et al. (2010). Acoustic overlap may suggest a relative lack of formant differentiation, imprecise articulatory configurations, and more vowel centralization (Mefferd & Green, 2010; Karlsson & van Doorn, 2012). A closer inspection of the vowel ellipses areas also showed that our participants with Broca's aphasia demonstrate greater dispersions in the high vowels /u:/ and /i:/ than in the low vowel /a:/ (Figures 2 and 5).

Taken together, the results of the present study would give more insight into the vowel acoustic characteristics of individuals with Broca's aphasia and expand our understanding of the underlying nature of Broca's aphasia. The results also suggest that vowels should become an integral part of speech therapy approaches and design.

For example, a speech therapist should include those vowels with more remarkable acoustic overlaps in their training and therapy activities to help patients to identify the differences between vowel pair affected by this acoustic overlap.

Limitations of the Study

The present study is limited by several aspects, including the number of participants, stimuli, and analysis scope. First, the sample size, which included eight individuals (six males and two females) with Broca's aphasia, was relatively small. Including more female speakers can also help determine the gender effect on formant frequencies and vowel durations, allowing for more comparisons between speaker groups.

Secondly, the speech stimuli are relatively small as it includes three vowels (/a:,u:,i). This number of vowels may not allow for general conclusions regarding vowel production in Arabic-speaking individuals with Boca's aphasics. However, the present study's findings are preliminary, and further research is needed to gain better understanding of vowel deficits in Broca's aphasia. Finally, the analysis did not address the formant trajectory and how formants change in time, which was found to be crucial for vowel duration (Liss & Weismer, 1992).

Conclusion

The present paper examined the acoustic features of vowel production in Palestinian Arabic-speakers with Broca's aphasics compared to healthy speakers. The findings showed speakers with Broca's aphasia produced all vowel categories with more variability compared to the healthy speakers. The results also revealed significantly higher F1 values for speakers with Broca's aphasics than the healthy speakers. Reduced vowel acoustic spaces and overlaps were noted in the speakers with Broca's aphasia. Such acoustic variability might reflect articulatory inconsistency and tongue positioning constraints.

Furthermore, although speakers with Broca's aphasia produced longer vowel durations than the healthy speakers, they were not significantly different between speaker groups. The present findings support previous studies concerning abnormal temporal and spectral patterns in Broca's aphasia. Acoustic measures of vowels can help speech therapists develop speech training programs to enhance speakers' speech intelligibility with Broca's aphasia.

References

1. Al-Ani, S. (1970). Arabic phonology; an acoustical and physiological investigation. The Hague: Mouton.

2. Abou Haidar, L. (1994). Norme linguistique et variabilitd dialectale: analyse formantique du systeme vocalique de la langue arabe. Revue de honetique Appliquee, 110, 1-15.

3. Alotaibi, Y. & Hussain, A. (2009). Formant based analysis of spoken Arabic vowels. Lecture Notes Computer Science, 5707, 162-169.

4. Aziz, M. A., Hassan, M., Razak, R., & Garraffa, M. (2020). Syntactic abilities in Malay adult speakers with aphasia: A study on passive sentences and argument structures. Aphasiology, 34(7), 886-904. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2020.1742283

5. Baum, S., & Boyczuk, J. (1999). Speech timing subsequent to brain damage: Effects of utterance length and complexity. Brain and Language, 67(1), 30 45. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1999.2047

6. Baum, S., & Pell, M. (1997). Production of affective and linguistic prosody by brain-damaged patients. Aphasiology, 11(2), 177-198. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039708248463

7. Belkaid, Y. (1984). Les voyelles de 1'arabe littdraire moderne. Analyse spectrographique. Travaux de 1'Institut de Phonetique de Strasbourg, 16, 217-240.

8. Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2020). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 1.43) [computer program]. http://www.praat.org.

9. Boye, K., & Bastiaanse, R. (2018). Grammatical versus lexical words in theory and aphasia: Integrating linguistics and neurolinguistics. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3, 1-18.

10. Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(1), 177-208. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1080/026432997381664

11. Chodroff, E., & Wilson, C. (2020). Acoustic-phonetic and auditory mechanisms of adaptation in the perception of sibilant fricatives. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(4), 20272048. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01894-2

12. Ferguson, S., & Kewley-Port, D. (2007). Talker differences in clear and conversational speech: Acoustic characteristics of vowels. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(5), 1241-1255. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/087)

13. Fyndanis, V., Arcara, G., Christidou, P., & Caplan, D. (2018). Morphosyntactic production and verbal working memory: Evidence from Greek aphasia and healthy aging. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(5), 1171-1187. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018 jslhr-l-170103

14. Gandour, J., & Dardarananda, R. (1984). Prosodic Disturbance in Aphasia: Vowel Length in Thai. Brain and Language, 23(2), 206-224. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934x(84)90064-6

15. Garraffa, M., & and Fyndanis, V. (2020). Linguistic theory and aphasia: an overview. Aphasiology, 34(2), 905-926. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2020.1770196

16. Haley, k., Bays, G. & Ohde, R. (2001). Phonetic properties of aphasic-apraxic speech: A modified narrow transcription analysis. Aphasiology, 15(12), 1125-1142. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687040143000519

17. Haley, K., Ohde, R. Wertz, T. (2011). Vowel quality in aphasia and apraxia of speech: Phonetic transcription and formant analyses. Aphasiology, 15, 1107-1123.

18. Johansson, M. (2012). Aphasia and Communication in Everyday Life: Experiences of persons with aphasia, significant others, and speech and language pathologists. Doctoral thesis of Uppsala University.

19. Karlsson F, & van Doorn, J. (2012). Vowel formant dispersion as a measure of articulation proficiency. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132(4), 26332641. https:ZZdoi.org/10.1121Z1.4746025

20. Keller, E. (1978). Parameters for vowel substitutions in Broca's aphasia. Brain and Language, 5(3), 265-285. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934x(78)90026-3

21. Kempler, D. & Van Lancker, D. (2002). Effect of Speech Task on Intelligibility in Dysarthria: A Case Study of Parkinson's Disease. Brain and Language, 80(3), 449464. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2602

22. Kurowski, K., Blumstein, S., Palumbo, C.,Waldstein, R., &Burton, M. (2007). Nasal Consonant Production in Broca's and Wernicke's Aphasics: Speech Deficits and Neuroanatomical

23. Correlates. Brain and Language, 100(3'), 262-275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.10.002

24. Lee, J., Shaiman, S., & Weismer, G. (2016). Relationship between tongue positions and formant frequencies in female speakers. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139(1), 426440. https:ZZdoi.org/10.1121Z1.4939894

25. Liu, H., Tsao, F., & Kuhl, P. (2005). The effect of reduced vowel working space on speech intelligibility in Mandarin-speaking young adults with cerebral palsy. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 117(6), 3879-3899. https://doi.org/10.112V1.1898623

26. Martinez-Ferreiro, S., Bastiaanse, R., & Boye, K. (2020). Functional and usage-based approaches to aphasia: The grammatical-lexical distinction and the role of frequency. Aphasiology, 34(8), 927-942. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2019.1615335

27. Matchin, W. (2018). A neuronal retuning hypothesis of sentence-specificity in Broca's area.

28. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(5), 1682-1694. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-0171377-6

29. Mefferd, A., & Green, J. (2010). The articulatory-to-acoustic relationship in response to speaking rate and loudness manipulations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(5), 1206-1219. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0083)

30. Menn, L., & Obler, L. (1990). Agrammatic Aphasia: A Cross-Language Narrative Sourcebook. Amsterdam und Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

31. Metoui, M. (1995). Phono Lab: Computerprogramm zur Artikulatorisch-Akustischen Datenanalyse. Arbeitsberichte des Instituts fur Allgemeine und Vergleichende Sprachwissenschaft der Universitdt Mainz, 1, 1-100.

32. Newman, D., & Verhoeven, J. (2002). Frequency analysis of Arabic in connected speech, Antwerp Papers in Linguistics, 100, 77-86.

33. Paradis, M. (1987). The assessment of bilingual aphasia. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

34. Ouden, D., Malyutina, S., Basilakos, A., Bonilha, L., Gleichgerrcht, E., Yourganov, G., Hillis, A.

35. E., Hickok, G., Rorden, C., & Fridriksson, J. (2019). Cortical and structural-connectivity damage correlated with impaired syntactic processing in aphasia. Human Brain Mapping, 40(7), 2153-2173. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24514

36. Ryalls, J. (1986). An acoustic study of vowel production in aphasia. Brain and Language, 29(1), 48-67. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934x(86)90033-7

37. Schimer, A. (2004). Timing speech: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(2), 269-287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.04.003

38. Seddoh, S. (2008). Conceptualisation of deviations in intonation production in aphasia.

39. Aphasiology, 22(12), 1294-1312. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030701640289

40. Tsao, Y., Weismer, G., & Iqubal, K. (2006). The effect of intertalker speech rate on acoustic vowel space. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(2), 10741082. https://doi.org/10.112V1.2149774

41. Verhaegen, C., Delvaux, V., Fagniart, S., Huet, K., Piccaluga, M. (2020). Phonological and phonetic impairment in aphasic speech: an acoustic study of the voice onset time of six French-speaking aphasic patients. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 34(3), 201221. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2019.1619095

42. Williams, S., & Seaver, J. (1986). A comparison of speech sound durations in three syndromes of aphasia. Brain and Language, 29(1), 171-182. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(86)90041-6

43. Ziegler, W., & Hoole, P. (1989). A combined acoustic and perceptual analysis of the tense-lax opposition in aphasic vowel production. Aphasiology, 3, 449-463.

Размещено на Allbest.ru

...

Подобные документы

  • Vowels and current English. Kinds of sound change. Early modern English pronunciation and spelling. Causes of sound change. Spelling: particular words. Pronunciation change and the great vowel shift. Some interpretation of the great vowel shift.

    диссертация [395,2 K], добавлен 22.01.2015

  • General guidelines on word stress: one word has only one stress; stress vowels, not consonants. Origins of the word stress and the notion of accent. English accentuation tendencies. Typical patterns of stress of nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs.

    курсовая работа [275,8 K], добавлен 12.04.2014

  • The Evolution of English. Vowels and current English. Kinds of Sound Change. Causes of sound change. The Phoneme and Differing transcriptions. Early modern English pronunciation and spelling. Vowel changes in Middle English and Early New English.

    дипломная работа [319,1 K], добавлен 20.01.2015

  • А complex comparison of morphological characteristics of English and Ukrainian verbs. Typological characteristics, classes and morphological categories of the English and Ukrainian verbs. The categories of person and number, tenses, aspect, voice, mood.

    дипломная работа [162,2 K], добавлен 05.07.2011

  • The concept and form preliminary investigation. Inquest: general provisions, the order of proceedings, dates. Preliminary and police investigation. Criminal procedural activities of the inquiry. Pre-trial investigation: investigative jurisdiction, terms.

    реферат [20,0 K], добавлен 14.05.2011

  • Definition of concept of slangy language. Consideration of the reasons of occurrence, history of an origin, phonetic peculiarities, morphological characteristics and types of slang (from the Internet, of army, police, money, cockeney rhyming, polary).

    курсовая работа [77,9 K], добавлен 06.02.2010

  • Can, may, We can compare may and can. Must. Must and May compared. To have to. To be to. Must, to have to and to be to compared. Ought to. Shall and should. Must, Should and ought to compared. Should + Perfect infinitive.

    дипломная работа [35,2 K], добавлен 22.07.2006

  • The problems as definition of nouns, main features of English nouns, their grammatical categories. Semantical characteristics of nouns and the category of number of english nouns. The lexicon-grammatical meaning of a class or of a subclass of words.

    курсовая работа [27,6 K], добавлен 07.07.2009

  • The influence of other languages and dialects on the formation of the English language. Changes caused by the Norman Conquest and the Great Vowel Shift.Borrowing and influence: romans, celts, danes, normans. Present and future time in the language.

    реферат [25,9 K], добавлен 13.06.2014

  • Historical background of the History of English. Assimilative Vowel Changes: Breaking and Diphthongisation. Old English phonetics and grammar. Morphological classification of nouns. Evolution of the grammatical system. Personal and possessive pronouns.

    курс лекций [104,6 K], добавлен 23.07.2009

  • Defining the notion "slang". Analyzing the use of slang in movies, literature, songs and Internet. Interviewing native American speakers. Singling out the classification of slang, its forms and characteristics. Tracing the origin and sources of slang.

    курсовая работа [73,6 K], добавлен 23.07.2015

  • The problem of category of number of nouns, Russian and English grammatical, syntactical and phonetic forms of expression. The general quantitative characteristics of words constitute the lexico-grammatical base for dividing the nounal vocabulary.

    контрольная работа [40,6 K], добавлен 25.01.2011

  • The concept of economic growth and development. Growth factors: extensive, intensive, the growth of the educational and professional level of personnel, improve the management of production. The factors of production: labor, capital and technology.

    презентация [2,3 M], добавлен 21.07.2013

  • Piracy around Somalia: how the pirates operate. Provide a coastguard for Somalia. Potential environmental catastrophe. Possible co-opting by international terrorist networks. Organize shipping into a safe lane. Anti-piracy measures, military presence.

    курсовая работа [29,8 K], добавлен 04.06.2009

  • The general notions of abbreviation in english. The history of abbreviations as long as phonetic script existed. The fashionable use of abbreviation - a kind of society slang. The importance of the issue for those who want to brush up on your English.

    курсовая работа [42,4 K], добавлен 29.12.2014

  • Phonetic coincidence and semantic differences of homonyms. Classification of homonyms. Diachronically approach to homonyms. Synchronically approach in studying homonymy. Comparative typological analysis of linguistic phenomena in English and Russia.

    курсовая работа [273,7 K], добавлен 26.04.2012

  • The subjects of stylistic phonetics, implemented by Percy Bysshe Shelley in his works. General morphology treats morphemes and grammatical meanings in language without regard to their stylistic value. The phonetic expressive means and stylistic devices.

    реферат [32,5 K], добавлен 20.11.2010

  • The concept and category values "voice" and "mood" in different languages. Features and comparative description of the use and formation of a voice in English and Armenian. Classification of moods, their characteristics of a grammatical point of view.

    курсовая работа [43,1 K], добавлен 06.10.2015

  • Modern sources of distributing information. Corpus linguistics, taxonomy of texts. Phonetic styles of the speaker. The peculiarities of popular science text which do not occur in other variations. Differences between academic and popular science text.

    курсовая работа [24,6 K], добавлен 07.02.2013

  • Definition and general characteristics of the word-group. Study of classification and semantic properties of the data units of speech. Characteristics of motivated and unmotivated word-groups; as well as the characteristics of idiomatic phrases.

    реферат [49,3 K], добавлен 30.11.2015

Работы в архивах красиво оформлены согласно требованиям ВУЗов и содержат рисунки, диаграммы, формулы и т.д.
PPT, PPTX и PDF-файлы представлены только в архивах.
Рекомендуем скачать работу.