I Want to Marry *the Merchant Banker: An Exploratory Self-Paced Reading Experiment on Arab Learners' Processing of English Articles

Explores the processing of English articles by native speakers of Hejazi Arabic, a language with articles. The research aims to answer the question of whether offline (explicit) knowledge of specificity in English article choice mirrors online knowledge.

Рубрика Иностранные языки и языкознание
Вид статья
Язык английский
Дата добавления 24.09.2023
Размер файла 361,6 K

Отправить свою хорошую работу в базу знаний просто. Используйте форму, расположенную ниже

Студенты, аспиранты, молодые ученые, использующие базу знаний в своей учебе и работе, будут вам очень благодарны.

Размещено на http://www.allbest.ru/

I Want to Marry *the Merchant Banker: An Exploratory Self-Paced Reading Experiment on Arab Learners' Processing of English Articles

Mona Sabir

Ph.D. in Applied Linguistics, Associate Professor

Alaa Melebari

Ph.D. in Linguistics, Assistant Professor

ABSTRACT

Purpose. This study explores the processing of English articles by native speakers of Hejazi Arabic, a language with articles. The research aims to answer the question of whether offline (explicit) knowledge of specificity in English article choice mirrors online (implicit) knowledge. Existing research has found that Hejazi-Arabic learners of English misuse articles in the indefinite specific context when answering a written task; however, their performance is target-like in all other contexts of article use, which indicates that their production may be sensitive to specificity, similar to the production of learners from languages that do not include articles. Little has been done, though, to explore this phenomenon using online measures.

Methods. To answer the research question, 68 speakers of Hejazi-Arabic were recruited alongside 23 native English speakers. The participants and native English speakers completed an article elicitation task and self-paced reading task.

Results. The results of the article elicitation task show learners' overuse of the in the indefinite specific context, which is consistent with the findings of existing research. Similarly, the real-time processing results indicate that there is a wider gap in reaction times between natives and L2 learners in the indefinite specific context, suggesting that learners' online performance is not on target in this context.

Conclusion. The study concludes that Hejazi Arabic speakers' online knowledge of English articles show some resemblance to their offline knowledge. Theoretical implications and methodological issues are also discussed.

Key words: processing of English articles, self-paced reading, definiteness, specificity, implicit knowledge, explicit knowledge.

АНОТАЦІЯ

reading experiment english articles

Мета. Це дослідження вивчає обробку англійських артиклів носіями хеджазької арабської мови, мови, в якій є артиклі. Дослідження має на меті відповісти на питання, чи є офлайнові (експліцитні) знання специфіки вибору англійського артикля дзеркальним відображенням онлайнових (імпліцитних) знань. Існуючі дослідження виявили, що геязи-араби, які вивчають англійську мову, неправильно вживають артиклі у невизначеному специфічному контексті, відповідаючи на письмове завдання; однак їхні результати є цільовими у всіх інших контекстах вживання артиклів, що вказує на те, що їхня продукція може бути чутливою до специфіки, подібно до продукції учнів з мов, які не мають артиклів. Однак, мало що було зроблено для вивчення цього явища за допомогою онлайн-вимірювань.

Методи. Щоб відповісти на дослідницькі питання, було набрано 68 носіїв хеджазі-арабської мови та 23 носії англійської мови. Учасники та носії англійської мови виконали завдання на пошук статей та завдання на самостійне читання.

Результати. Результати завдання на пошук статей свідчать про надмірне вживання учнями неозначеного відмінка в конкретному контексті, що узгоджується з висновками існуючих досліджень. Аналогічно, результати обробки в реальному часі вказують на більший розрив у часі реакції між носіями мови та тими, хто вивчає мову L2, у невизначеному конкретному контексті, що свідчить про те, що в цьому контексті студенти не досягають бажаних результатів.

Висновки. Дослідження показало, що онлайн-знання англомовних статей хеджазькою арабською мовою демонструють певну схожість з їхніми знаннями в офлайні. Також обговорюються теоретичні висновки та методологічні питання.

Ключові слова: опрацювання англомовних статей, самостійне читання, визначеність, специфічність, імпліцитні знання, експліцитні знання.

Introduction

English article acquisition has received significant attention over the last 20 years (Garda Mayo & Hawkins, 2009). The consensus on L2 learners' production is that article substitution errors (i.e. using the instead of a/an, and vice versa) are more persistent than article omission errors, especially among learners from article-less (-ART) languages. Ionin et al. (2004) offered a theoretical explanation for substitution errors in non-generic contexts by attributing the substitution errors common among learners from -ART language backgrounds to the specificity feature, but they left the question regarding learners from a language background with articles (+ART) unanswered.

A limited number of studies have used psycholinguistic or online approaches to explore L2 learners' article use (Ionin et al., 2021). It is essential to use these methods because they draw upon implicit knowledge more than offline tasks, which draw upon explicit knowledge (Ellis, 2005; Jegerski, 2014; Jiang, 2007). It has been found that adult L2 learners generally perform better at offline tasks that target explicit knowledge than at online tasks that target implicit knowledge (Ellis, 2005). Though online measures may be reliable indicators of learners' implicit knowledge, they do have pitfalls (Ionin et al., 2021). For example, learners' omission errors in tasks that explore implicit knowledge may be due to the pressure of these timed tasks rather than a lack of knowledge. Psycholinguistic methods, such as self-paced reading tasks (SPRTs), help to circumvent this limitation. Drawing upon implicit knowledge, these methods assess comprehension without placing learners under production pressure, as Jiang (2007) discussed. Hence, this study compared performance in an SPRT versus an offline task to investigate whether the implicit knowledge of English articles among L2 English learners from an L1 with articles (Hejazi Arabic) mirrors their explicit knowledge.

Previous research focused on the production of English articles by Arabic-speaking learners (+ART language background) (Abumelha, 2016; Almahboob, 2009; Sabir, 2015; Sabir, 2018) found that Arabic learners often overuse the in contexts where definiteness and specificity are not aligned. These speakers' production was found to be sensitive to the specificity feature in the indefinite specific context, similar to learners of -ART L1s. This behaviour was observed offline in written tasks. English language learners were generally found to use articles more accurately when completing imitation tasks (where half of the target sentences were missing articles) than in forced choice elicitation tasks, which suggests that tasks that rely on explicit knowledge direct learners' attention towards explicit grammatical rules (Ionin, personal communication). Therefore, if learners from +ART language backgrounds fluctuate between the and a/an (i.e. are sensitive to specificity) in tasks that are assumed to explore explicit knowledge, a plausible explanation may be that these learners do not rely on implicit (intuitive) knowledge (i.e. their behaviour cannot be explained in terms of parameter setting) but instead rely on explicit (and linguistically invalid) beliefs.

Arabic-speaking learners' online processing of English articles has not yet been explored. In light of the findings of Ionin et al. (2004) and Ionin et al. (2021), the present study compared the role of specificity in the English article choice of Hejazi Hejaz refers to the western region of Saudi Arabia (western border on the Red Sea). Arabic speakers both offline (using an article elicitation task [AET]) and online (using an SPRT). Therefore, the paper aims to explore whether implicit knowledge of articles among Arabic-speaking L2 learners of English mirrors their explicit knowledge.

The paper begins with a background of Ionin et al.'s (2004) work, followed by a contrastive analysis of definiteness and specificity marking in Arabic compared to English, and a brief overview of the existing research on English article acquisition and processing. The details of the current study are provided next. Finally, the results of the study are presented and discussed.

Theoretical Background

Ionin et al.'s (2004) proposal is centered on two semantic features that are considered to be discourse-related properties of articles, namely definiteness and specificity. Ionin et al. (2004) found that article misuse was related to these two features in learners' L1 backgrounds. The present study adopted Ionin et al.'s (2004) definitions of definiteness (shared knowledge between the speaker and hearer) and specificity (speaker's knowledge), based on Heim (1991) and Fodor and Sag (1982), respectively. The definitions are as follows:

If a determiner phrase (DP) in the form [D NP] is...

a. [+definite], then the speaker and hearer presuppose the

existence of a unique individual in the set denoted by the NP.

b. [+specific], then the speaker intends to refer to a unique

individual in the set denoted by the NP and considers this

individual to possess some noteworthy property.

(Ionin et al., 2004: 5).

The key notions of Ionin et al.'s (2004) proposal are the article choice parameter (ACP) and the fluctuation hypothesis (FH). The ACP dictates article choice in two-article languages cross-linguistically; the parameter has two settings: Languages either encode articles based on definiteness (e.g. standard English and Arabic) or based on specificity (e.g. Samoan).

By analysing data collected from Russian and Korean English learners (from -ART languages), Ionin et al. (2004) found that these

learners may have access to UG parameters and that their article choice may consequently fluctuate between the two parameter settings. This fluctuation is the core concept of the FH, under which L2 learners' fluctuation results in substitution errors that may be detected in contexts where there is a mismatch between definiteness and specificity. Therefore, learners may use theinstead of a/an in [-definite; +specific] contexts and a/an instead of the in [+definite; -specific] contexts according to the specificity setting. On the other hand, learners may not substitute articles when there is no clash between definiteness and specificity ([+definite; +specific] and [-definite; -specific]). Ionin et al. (2009) revised the original FH after evaluating Samoan articles and limited fluctuation to the overuse of the in [-definite; +specific] contexts. The predictions for article choice are presented in Table 1.

Table 1

Predictions for Article Choice in L2 English (revised version of Ionin et al.'s (2004: 19))

[+definite]: target the

[-definite]: target a

+specific

correct use of the

overuse of the

-specific

correct use of the

correct use of a

According to the FH, native speakers of -ART languages never set the ACP when acquiring their L1, which causes them to fluctuate between the specificity and the definiteness setting when acquiring an L2 with articles. Despite the fact that this parameter setting is an unconscious, implicit process, most research on the FH has used methods that draw upon explicit knowledge (e.g. forced-choice elicitation tasks), warranting an investigation of the implicit processing of articles in L2 learners.

How Arabic and English Mark Definiteness and Specificity

Arabic and English are similar in terms of definiteness-based article encoding. However, Arabic differs from English in terms of marking indefinites in that the former allows indefinite bare NPs. According to Bardeas (2009), spoken varieties of Hejazi Arabic do not have an overt marker for indefiniteness (Example 1).

To mark definiteness, Arabic has the morphological marker al- in both its standard and non-standard varieties, whereas English has the markerthe. These markers are used in both languages' written and spoken varieties. Furthermore, the andal- are not marked for number or gender, as is the case in several other +ART languages (e.g. Spanish).

(1). Bait

house.M.SG `a house'

(Bardeas, 2009: 31).

Definiteness in Arabic is not realised solely using -al (see Example 2), which is attached to nouns as a prefix. It is also indicated in other forms such as a noun construct relationship called idaafa, in which the first noun is considered definite because it is related to a second noun that is definite (Example 3 in italics). Another way to realise definiteness is by using a possessive suffix (Example 4; Sarko, 2011). It should be noted that Examples 1-5 are from modern standard Arabic (MSA), while Examples 6-10 are from the Hejazi Arabic dialect.

(2) . ?usaafiru?ilaa dimafq-ibi-al-sajjaarat-idaa?im-an

travel.I.PRESToDamascus-GENby-the-car.F-GENalways-ACC

`I always travel to Damascus by car.'

(3) .?stairtukitaab-a

borrow.I.PASTbook-ACC

`I borrowed the boy's book.'

(4) .?stairtukitaab-uh

borrow.I.PASTBook-his

`I borrowed his book.'

(Sarko, 2011: 25-26).

This study focuses on simple definites (Example 1), which are expressed through the presence or absence of al-, as supported by Lyons (1999: 2), as follows:

Noun phrases with the and a and their semantic equivalent (or near-equivalents) in other languages can be thought of as the basic instantiations of definite and indefinite noun phrases, in that the definiteness or indefiniteness stems from the presence of the article, which has as its essential semantic function to express this category.

To mark indefiniteness in written and spoken varieties, English uses a/an with singular NPs and null with plural NPs. Conversely, it has been argued that the suffix -n (nunation) is the indefinite marker in MSA (Ryding, 2005). The reason underlying this argument is that nunation appears in a complementary distribution with the definite prefix al- (Example 5). However, scholars such as Lyons (1999) and Almahboob (2009) have rejected this argument (Example 6); the latter has argued that in MSA, “nunation is permissible with a proper noun, which is considered definite, indicating that if nunation were a true marker of indefiniteness, it is feasible to think that it should not occur with a definite noun” (Almahboob, 2009: 53).Brustad (2000), Holes (1995), and Ryding (2005) have provided more linguistic details about the Arabic DP.

(5) . baab-u-n

door.M.SG-NOM-INDIF `a door'

(Bardeas, 2009: 29).

(6) . hind-u-n

Hind.M-NOM-Nunation

`Hind' (a proper noun female name)

(Almahboob, 2009: 52).

On the contrary, the specificity distinction in Arabic (which is similar to that in English), is not morphologically realised; rather, it is expressed in discourse (Sarko, 2011). Sarko (2011) founded this argument on the results of an investigation of Syrian Arabic. The realisation of specificity through context is illustrated in the following examples from Hejazi Arabic, which show four contexts of the combination of the features ±definite ±specific (Examples 7-10). In Examples 7 and 9, there is no marker of specificity, though it is clear from the context that the speaker has a specific referent in mind. On the other hand, Examples 8 and 10 show that the speaker is not thinking about a specific referent.

(7).

[+definite, +specific]

aftaray-t

al-galam

hag

maadat

al-rasam

bought-ISG

DEF-pen

for

course

DEF-art

`I bought the pen for the art course.'

(8).

[+definite, -specific]

mudeer

al-madrasa

ha-yiitH

al-jayizah

li

head

DEF-school

will-present

DEF-prize

To

al-fayiz

fi

musabagat

al-rasam

DEF-winner

In

Contest

DEF-art

`The head teacher will present the prize to the winner

of the art contest.'

(9).

[-definite, +specific]

?abya

aftari

kitaab

ian

al-t!abx

want-lSG

buy

Book

About

DEF-cooking

lakan

sairu

yali

But

price-3SG

expensive

`I want to buy a cookery book, but it

is expensive.'

(10).

[-definite, -specific]

?abya

aftari

kitaab

ian

al-t!abx

want 1SG

buy 1SG

Book

About

DEF-cooking

lakan

ma-a?raf

aif

Axtaar

But

not-know

Which

choose-lSG

`I want to buy a cookery book, but I don't know which one to choose.'

(Sabir, 2018: 142-143).

In summary, the distinction between definite and indefinite structures is marked morphosyntactically in both English and Arabic. In contrast, neither language expresses the distinction between specific and non-specific morphosyntactically; rather, the interpretation depends on contextual factors. In other words, whether an NP is interpreted as having a specific or a non-specific reference depends on the context.

The Acquisition of English Articles

The practice of viewing article acquisition according to ACP, FH, and UG accessibility started with Ionin et al.'s (2004) study in which the authors tested the article choice of 30 L1 speakers of Russian and 40 L1 speakers of Korean during a forced choice elicitation task. Their study produced clear evidence of fluctuation between definiteness and specificity. To gain further insights into the topic, Ionin et al. (2009) compared English article choice among adult and child speakers of Russian. Whereas Ionin et al. (2004) generalised sensitivity to specificity to both definite and indefinite contexts, Ionin et al. (2009) found that the specificity effect was limited to indefinites, as stated in Table 1. Moreover, Ionin et al. (2009) associated learners' sensitivity to specificity with their language proficiency levels. In other words, the authors found sensitivity to specificity exclusively regarding indefinites in the production of learners with higher proficiency levels. Conversely, lower proficiency learners tended to show sensitivity to specificity with both definites and indefinites.

Ionin et al. (2004) did not test the production of learners from +ART language backgrounds, which has motivated other researchers to investigate whether such learners' article choice will fluctuate. Garcia Mayo (2009) targeted low-intermediate and advanced L1 Spanish learners of English using the same elicitation task, found no evidence of fluctuation, and concluded that transfer overrides fluctuation. Additionally, Hawkins et al. (2006) conducted a comparative study involving learners from +ART language backgrounds whose L1 encodes definiteness (Greek) and learners from -ART language backgrounds (Japanese). Using the same elicitation task as Ionin et al. (2004), the authors found that the Greek speakers did not fluctuate in [-definite, +specific] contexts, a finding consistent with those of Garcia Mayo (2009). Furthermore, Snape (2006) compared article choice in L1 Spanish and L1 Japanese learners of L2 English and found that L1 Japanese learners made substitution errors, which, according to Snape, was not the result of inappropriate parameter resetting, but rather due to the remapping features of the UG/L1 to L2 forms. In contrast, Snape found that Spanish learners did not substitute articles, a finding in line with the full transfer/full access hypothesis (FTFA; Schwartz & Sprouse, 1996).

Regarding Arabic-speaking learners of English, Sarko (2009) tested French and Syrian Arabic speakers, both of whom have +ART L1 backgrounds where articles are assigned based on definiteness; however, these two languages differ from English and from one another in their morpho-phonological distribution of articles. The author hypothesised that if the representation of indefinites contains D, spoken Arabic has a phonologically null exponent, and fluctuation is (not) expected. However, if D is absent in the representation of indefinites, spoken Arabic indefinites would have a status similar to that of NPs in -ART languages, causing Arabic learners' article choice to fluctuate. The results show that the Syrian Arabic group differed significantly from the NS control group in [-definite, +specific] contexts. Sarko argued that this result does not support the FH and that Syrian Arabic learners used the mainly in contexts where relative clauses were present, which is consistent with the FTFA but inconsistent with the FH. Thus, Sarko's study created an opportunity to test Arab learners' article choice in [-definite, +specific] contexts where relative clauses are not present (i.e. in which the L1 transfer effect is not expected), which will reveal whether Arabic-speaking learners are sensitive to the effect of specificity, in line with the focus of the present study.

In addition to Sarko (2009), Almahboob (2009) tested Arabic-speaking learners of English using a forced-choice elicitation task and a written production task and found that these learners fluctuated between the two parameter settings of the ACP in the [-definite, +specific] context. Additionally, Sabir (2018) conducted a study involving 58 Hejazi Arabic speakers to determine whether specificity influenced English article choice (excluding relative clause modification structures) using Ionin et al.'s (2009) AET. Hejazi Arabic-speaking learners fluctuated between definiteness and specificity in the same way as learners from -ART language backgrounds, a finding that contradicts existing research on L2 learners from +ART L1 backgrounds (Garcia Mayo, 2009; Hawkins et al., 2006; Sarko, 2009; Snape, 2006). Sabir (2018) linked these findings to Arabic articles' linguistic characteristics such as the lack of an indefinite marker or the possible existence of a specificity marker in the spoken variety. However, further research is required to investigate the presence of such a marker.

Previous Online Studies on Articles in the L2

Most of the research conducted on L2 learners' article usage has focused on their offline production and neglected mapping it to L2 learners' implicid knowledge. Furthermore, the few studies that have examined L2 learners' implicit knowledge have largely relied on data from learners from -ART language backgrounds. Kim and Lakshmanan (2009) are among the few that have utilised psycholinguistic approaches; they investigated the ability of Korean-speaking L2 learners of English to distinguish English articles based on specificity rather than definiteness. Using an SPRT, the authors presented participants with pairs of sentences that included either the definite article the or the indefinite article a, in which the first sentence contained the selected article and the second denoted context (specific vs. non-specific). Since the authors focused on specificity, they only created two conditions: a ([-definite, -specific] condition and a [(-definite, +specific] condition appearing in a grammatical and an ungrammatical version. The participants were placed in either the intermediate or the advanced group based on their English proficiency level. The results suggest that the control group and the advanced L2 learners fluctuated between the definiteness setting and the specificity setting, whereas intermediate L2 learners initially processed articles based on specificity and then definiteness, which is in line with Ionin et al.'s (2004) proposal. Kim and Lakshmanan's (2009) results were inconclusive; learners' and native speakers' reading times (RTs) were not distinct from each other, and the total RT for the second sentence may have been inaccurate.

Ahn (2019) further explored the processing of English articles by L1 Korean speakers of L2 English. Using an SPRT, the author investigated the ways in which L1 and L2 speakers differ in terms of processing definite vs. indefinite NPs that presuppose unique vs. non-unique referents. Participants were presented with sentences including a definite or an indefinite noun in either a unique or non-unique context; the sentences were divided into eight to nine regions (with three regions of interest) to compare L1 vs. L2 participants' behaviour and track significant effects. The findings show that the L2 participants (intermediate and advanced) initially resorted to their L1 grammar; they demonstrated sensitivity to definiteness in their longer reaction times in the `critical region', whereas the advanced group manifested another latter target-like effect in their longer `post-spill-over region' reaction times. According to the author, the L2 intermediate and advanced groups' long reaction times in the critical region may be interpreted as “real-time evidence for [the] FH” (Ahn, 2019: 20).

Trenkic et al. (2013) investigated Mandarin-speaking (-ART) L2 English learners' online use of articles using the eye-tracking method. The study focused on whether L2 learners, like native speakers, could choose between the anda to resolve reference. To illustrate, the learners and the native control group heard sentences such as The pirate will put the cube inside the/a can, while looking at open and closed cans, where only the open can was an accurate reference relating to the cube. Trenkic et al. (2013) found that learners behaved like native speakers. That is, when participants heard the can, they chose one open can; on the other hand, if they heard a can, they chose two open cans. Trenkic et al. (2013) attributed this result to the association between the and uniqueness. This result indicates that the L1 Mandarin learners of L2 English successfully associated the articles the and a with the concept of (non)uniqueness.

Furthermore, in Agebjorn (2020), L2 acquisition of English articles among 26 participants from -ART language (Russian and Belarusian) backgrounds were investigated. Learners' explicit knowledge (i.e. their ability to explicate the principles governing the distinction between definite and indefinite structures) was tested using a multiple choice task, while their implicit knowledge was tested using an oral communicative task. Having compared the results of both tasks, Agebjorn found no correlation between explicit and implicit knowledge, suggesting that explicit knowledge is not crucial for these learners' acquisition of article meaning.

Ionin et al. (2021) used both offline and online methods to explore whether L2 learners of English from a Mandarin (-ART) language background could acquire the indefinite article despite the lack of articles in their L1. The authors examined learners' sensitivity to the two types of article errors, namely omission and misuse. Thirty-two native English speakers and 32 Mandarin-speakers of L2 English participated in an SPRT and a grammaticality judgement task. The results show that learners were sensitive to article errors online and that their performance was more on target online than offline.

In summary, most of the studies that have explored the online acquisition of English articles examined data from learners with -ART language backgrounds. To the best of our knowledge, no study has utilised psycholinguistic approaches to investigate knowledge of the L2 article system among learners from +ART language backgrounds. Studies such as Trenkic et al. (2013) have shown that L2 learners can acquire articles implicitly. Our study used an SPRT method similar to that employed by Kim and Lakshmanan (2009), Ahn (2019), and

Ionin et al. (2021) to explore whether Hejazi Arabic (+ART) speakers' implicit knowledge of specificity in the English article system mirrors their explicit knowledge. It is hypothesised that explicit knowledge of definiteness will reflect implicit knowledge, as it is grammaticised in the learners' L1, whereas explicit knowledge of specificity will not reflect implicit knowledge, as it is not grammaticised in their L1.

The Current Study

This study aims to explore whether native speakers of Hejazi Arabic are affected by specificity when choosing English articles offline and/or online. Based on the findings of Almahboob (2009) and Sabir (2018), this research made certain predictions. First, in an AET, learners' article choice is expected to fluctuate in the indefinite specific context since they are being tested offline. That is, these learners are expected to overuse the (instead of a) in the indefinite specific context. Second, in an SPRT and based on studies on the event-related potentials (ERPs) of semantic anomalies (Holcomb, 1988; Holcomb & Neville, 1990; Kutas & Hillyard, 1980, 1984, 1989; Rugg, 1985, 1987), a semantic mismatch between the article and its context is expected to slow processing, yielding longer latencies. Therefore, the use of a orthe in an incongruent context would require longer processing time. However, given that the mismatch cases provided in this study are based on whether the article is used in felicitous vs. infelicitous (rather than grammatical vs. ungrammatical) contexts, a significant slowdown is not expected to occur in the mismatch cases. In contrast, significant differences between native speakers (NSs) and L2 learners (L2ers) are predicted; specifically, NSs are expected to outpace L2ers at processing both match and mismatch cases, since they process L1 information, unlike their L2er counterparts (Martin et al., 2013; Perez & Bajo, 2018).

The current study's central prediction is that if participants show sensitivity towards the target context (the indefinite specific) in terms of fluctuation (monitored by the AET) and longer latencies (monitored by the SPRT) compared to NSs, it may be concluded that they have not acquired the English article system either explicitly or implicitly. Contrastively, if the participants show no sensitivity to specificity in the AET and the SPRT, i.e. they have target-like latencies, the assumption is that the system has been acquired implicitly and explicitly. That is, fluctuation in the AET corresponds to failure to acquire explicit knowledge, whereas longer latencies and non-target-like behaviour in the SPRT correspond to failure to acquire implicit knowledge.

Method

Participants

The participants comprised 68 L1 Hejazi Arabic-speaking learners of English (L2ers; mean age = 20.2) and 23 native speakers of English (NSs; mean age = 20). The NSs were graduate and undergraduate students recruited from an American university. The Hejazi Arabic speakers were also university students taking a compulsory English as a foreign language (EFL) course. These participants were recruited through their academic institution. The L2ers completed a language background questionnaire in addition to the experimental tasks and reported that Hejazi Arabic was their only L1 and that they were learning English as their L2. Most learners started learning English at the intermediate school stage as per the national school curriculum. Additionally, the questionnaire revealed that these participants had never spent any period longer than a month in an English-speaking country and that they mainly used English in language classrooms or on social media sites. Based on their Oxford Quick Proficiency Test (OQPT) scores, the participants were classified as lower intermediate-level learners of English (i.e. participants scored between 24 and 30 out of 40 on Part 1 of the OQPT and between 30 and 39 out of 60 on Part 2). Their level per the Council of Europe was B1.

Instruments and Procedure

To test the abovementioned predictions, the study adopted Ionin et al.'s (2009) AET and an SPRT specifically designed for this experiment (given that the SPRT is considered a measure of implicit knowledge; Jegerski, 2014; Jiang, 2007). The AET and the SPRT were both administered in one session. The SPRT was hosted on Ibex Farm (http://spellout.net/ibexfarm/; Drummond, 2011), an online platform for psycholinguistic experiments. The EFL participants took, on average, 1 hour to complete the AET and the OQPT, while the English NSs took, on average, 15 minutes to finish the AET. Both groups completed the SPRT in approximately 25-35 minutes. The tests were administered to the participants in the following order: the OQPT, followed by the SPRT, and then the AET. All participants (NSs and L2ers) were asked to complete a demographic information form and provide consent at the beginning of the experiment. Participants were compensated for their time and effort.

AET

The AET comprised 48 short dialogues targeting the use of articles with singular count nouns. The dialogues included 24 target items (six for each target context) covering non-generic contexts involving definiteness and specificity (see Examples 13-16) and 24 fillers. The test was prefaced by an instruction page explaining how to answer the questions; this was followed by a practice question. For the full AET dataset, see Sabir and Melebari (2023a).

(11) . [+definite, +specific]

At a bookstore

Chris: Well, I've bought everything that I wanted. Are you ready to go?

Mike: Almost. Can you please wait a few minutes? I want to talk to

owner of this bookstore - she is a very nice lady, and I always say hi to her.

(12) . [+definite, -specific]

After a girls' soccer game at school Child: Excuse me! Can you please let me in?

Coach: What do you need?

Child: I am a reporter for my school newspaper! I need to talk towinner

of this game - I don't know who she is, so can you please help me?

(13) . [-definite, +specific]

Father comes home

Father: Thank you for taking care of Karen. How did you spend the day?

Babysitter: Well, we went to a park. Karen played in the sandbox for a while.

And then she metbeautiful, friendly boy - he was very well behaved,

and Karen played with him for almost an hour.

(14) . [-definite, -specific]

After school

Father: Do you have any homework?

Child: Yes, I need to write a book report.

Father: So what will you read?

Child: Hmm... I don't know yet. But I like to read about things that move - cars, trains... I know! I would like to readbook about airplanes! I'll go to the library tomorrow!

SPRT

For the SPRT, 16 of the 48 dialogues used in the AET were selected based on length and simplicity and rotated across the same four contexts, in addition to the manipulation of grammaticality. That is, each dialogue appeared once in a match condition and once in a mismatch condition in all contexts. To create the mismatch conditions, this study crossed the article choice factor with the article context factor (see Example 15).

(15) . [+Definite, +Specific]

At a bookstoreMatch condition

Chris: Well, I've bought everything that I wanted. Are you (article choice = the) ready to go?

Mike: Almost. Can you please wait a few minutes? I want to talk to a very nice lady who I always say hi to. She is the manager of this bookstore.

At a bookstoreMismatch condition

Chris: Well, I've bought everything that I wanted. Are you (article choice=a) ready to go?

Mike: Almost. Can you please wait a few minutes? I want to talk to a very nice lady who I always say hi to. She is *a manager of this bookstore.

The rotation of items, along with the generation of matched and mismatched versions of each item, yielded eight lists in total. The list had a between-item design in which, for instance, Participant 1 would see half the items from Condition A and half the items from Condition B, and Participant 2 would see exactly the opposite; each participant saw each item only once.

It is important to note that in each dialogue, the experimental manipulation appeared in the last sentence in more than 90.0% of the items. That is, the last sentence established the context of the critical NP. In some cases, it was impossible to maintain the location of the experimental manipulation without altering semantic context, which resulted in the remaining 10.0% of the items. Moreover, since the dialogues were adopted from Ionin et al. (2009), the number of words in each item varied, as any adjustments intended to make the number of words across all items uniform would likely have impacted the semantic context. Therefore, this study prioritised maintaining the proper semantic context for each article over the number of words and their location in each experimental item.A possible solution would have been to follow Kim and Lakshman's (2009) unique method of calculating RTs for all words in each item in the same context, calculating the total, and then computing the mean for each item. However, their method could not be applied to this research given the much higher word number variance. Ignoring the variance would have resulted in masking the actual results for each condition. Considering this factor, this study marked the noun that followed the article as the critical region (Region 1) and the sentence-final word as the spill-over region (Region 2). Both regions appeared in the final sentence of each experimental item. Examples (16) and (17) below illustrate this point.

(16) . [+Definite, -Specific]

At the end of a foot raceMatch

Laura: Are you ready to leave?article choice = the

Betsy: No, not yet. I don't know who is playing today. I'm writing a report about this, and I need to talk to the winner of this race.

(17) . [+Definite, +Specific]

Grandfather comes for a visitMatch

Grandfather: Where is my little granddaughter, Beth? Is she article choice = the home?

Father: No, she is not going to be back until late. She is having dinner with her best friend. It's Angie, the girl from her class.

Participants were randomly assigned to one of the eight lists, such that each list was presented approximately eight times for the L2ers and three times for the NSs. The experimental items on each list were counterbalanced using Ibex Farm's counterbalance feature to ensure that the items were shuffled and reordered for the participants.

After each experimental item, participants were presented with a yes/no comprehension question to ensure that they were paying adequate attention to the task. Participants were seated in a quiet lab approximately 25 cm from a computer monitor. All experimental items were presented on a white screen in black font, except for the yes/no options that appeared after the comprehension questions, which were in blue font. The task started with a white page that provided simple instructions for the experiment, followed by a practice question. Participants were then asked to begin whenever they were ready. The actual experiment started with blank spaces for the equivalent words in each experimental item displayed in the middle of the screen, and participants were asked to use the spacebar to make the words appear. Short conversations were presented one word at a time. Each press of the spacebar caused the previous word to disappear (see Fig. 1).

The yes/no comprehension questions following each experimental item were unrelated to the experimental manipulation and were only included to ensure that participants were not distracted. To ensure that the experimental items were well masked and that the participants could not make any generalisations, the correct answers to the comprehension questions were equally split between `yes' and `no'. The participants were also presented with the same 24 fillers from the AET, in addition to the experimental items. Participants were directed to use their mouse to click on yes/no to answer the comprehension questions, as illustrated in Figure 2.

Figure 1

Session Illustration for the one-word-at-a-time Presentation of the SPRT

Figure 2

Session Illustration for the Comprehension Questions in the SPRT

Using the embedded Ibex Farm feature, we measured reaction times for all the words presented to participants and then extracted words/regions of interest. We then compared the reaction times across the four contexts and between groups in both the match and mismatch conditions. For the full SPRT dataset, see Sabir and Melebari (2023b).

Data Analyses

To explore the effect of specificity in the AET, analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were run for the L2ers and the NS control group regarding the use of the and a/an. For the SPRT, ANOVAs were run to explore article choice (the vs.a/an) in each article context (definite specific, definite nonspecific, indefinite specific, and indefinite nonspecific) as independent variables, with (RT) as a dependent variable. For each context, there was the condition of matched (felicitous) vs. mismatched (infelicitous) article choice, creating a 2x4x2 design.

Results

AET

In each test context, each participant was given a total score (out of six). Overall, the AET descriptive data show that learners' scores were relatively low in the indefinite specific context compared to the other three contexts, as can be seen in Table 2.

Table 2

AET Descriptive Results for L2ers and NSs Context

DefiniteDefiniteIndefiniteIndefinite specificnon-specific Specificnon-specific

Group

N

M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

L1 Hejazi Arabic

68

4.59

1.55

3.94

1.44

3.63

1.51

4.71

1.44

learners of L2 English

Native control

23

6

0

5.69

.47

5.78

.52

5.61

.66

Note: N = number; M = mean; SD = standard deviation

Separate repeated-measures (RM) ANOVAs were conducted on the use of the vs. a/an for the L2ers and the NS control group. The NSs' results, as reported in Table 3, show that they performed as expected. The effect of definiteness was found to be highly significant, indicating that a clear distinction between [+definite] and [- definite] in terms of article use. In contrast, there was no statistical significance for the specificity variable or for crossing definiteness with specificity when testing the use of a. As expected, there was no fluctuation in article choice between a and the. Statistical significance was found when definiteness was crossed with specificity when testing the use of the, which suggests that NSs may be selecting the incorrect article due to failure to focus on the task, particularly because the semantic context manipulation was placed towards the end of each dialogue.

The L2ers' results, as shown in Table 4, indicate that the effect of definiteness is highly significant. The effect of specificity on the use of both the anda/an is also highly significant. Therefore, L2ers fluctuated between the anda/an. The interaction between definiteness and specificity was also significant.

Table 3

Results of RM ANOVAs for the NS Control Group

Factor

Use of the

Use of a

Df

F value

Adjusted p value

Df

F value

Adjusted p value

Definiteness

1

5808

< .001

1

3480

<.001

Specificity

1

.518

.58

1

.042

.84

Definiteness x specificity

1

9.86

< .01

1

2.78

.11

Table 4

Results of RM ANOVAs for L2ers

Factor

Use of the

Use of a

Df

F value

Adjusted p value

Df

F value

Adjusted p value

Definiteness

1

25959

<.001

1

67353

<.001

Specificity

1

50.82

<.001

1

310

<.001

Definiteness x specificity

1

3.98

<.05

1

219

<.001

SPRT

The data analysed in this section were obtained from the two regions that were selected based on where we expected to see the processing cost. Given that L2 learners would need to process the noun that follows the article in order to detect a mismatch, we identified this as the critical region (Region 1), and given that the sentence-final word marked the end of a complete thought, it was selected as the spill-over region (Region 2) in order to catch any spill-over effect (Roberts 2003; Roberts & Lizka, 2013). This region was selected to keep the experimental design consistent since function words separated the noun that followed the article and the sentence-final word.

SPRT Descriptive Statistics

The SPRT descriptive results reveal differences in reaction times between the NSs and L2ers in all four contexts in the two regions under match and mismatch conditions (see Table 5). Overall, L2ers' reaction times were much longer than NSs'. Furthermore, NSs had longer reaction times under the mismatch conditions (with a few exceptions), whereas L2ers showed variation under the match and mismatch conditions across the four contexts.

Table 5

Mean Reaction Times (in ms) Under the Match and Mismatch Conditions for NSs and L2ers Across the four Contexts in Regions 1 and 2

Descriptive statistics

Match condition

Mismatch condition

Native speakers

L2 learners

Native speakers

L2 learners

Region

1

Region

2

Region

1

Region

2

Region

1

Region

2

Region

1

Region

2

[+definite, +specific]

280

400

607

686

300

461

613

630

[+definite, -specific]

308

500

622

741

327

481

641

833

[-definite, -specific]

384

455

659

690

382

484

730

806

[-definite, +specific]

330

300

730

680

417

550

670

841

By combining the match and mismatch conditions across grammaticality (Table 6), this study compared each group's longest reaction times. For NSs, the longest reaction time was 382 ms in the [-definite, -specific] context in Region 1 and 490 ms in the [+definite, -specific] context in Region 2. On the other hand, the longest reaction time for L2ers was 700 ms in the [-definite, +specific] context in Region 1, but this effect changed in Region 2, in which L2ers exhibited more target-like behaviour, with the longest reaction time at 786 ms in the [+definite, -specific] context, similar to NSs. Figures 3 and 4 visually present these effects.

Table 6

Mean Reaction Times (in ms) and Standard Deviations for NSs and L2ers Across the four Contexts in Regions 1 and 2 Combining Match and Mismatch Conditions

Descriptive statistics (match and mismatch combined)

Region 1

Region 2

Native speakers

L2 learners

Native speakers

L2 learners

Mean RT

SD

Mean RT

SD

Mean RT

SD

Mean RT

SD

[+definite, +specific]

291

130

610

239

430

369

658

278

[+definite, -specific]

318

161

631

275

490

227

786

277

[-definite, -specific]

382

145

695

272

469

169

768

262

[-definite, +specific]

373

162

700

229

425

189

761

276

Figure 3

Mean Reaction Times and Standard Deviations Between NSs and L2ers in Region 1

Figure 4

Mean Reaction Times and Standard Deviations Between NSs and L2ers in Region 2

To explore whether L2ers' reaction times were on target, this research examined the absolute difference in reaction times between L2ers and NSs in each context in both the critical and the spill-over regions. The graph in Figure 5 shows that the widest difference between the two groups was in the [-definite, +specific] context.

Figure 5

Absolute Difference in Reaction time Between NSs and L2ers in Regions 1 and 2

SPRT Inferential Statistics

Numerous ANOVAs were run to identify the effects and determine whether they supported the research predictions. The first analysis examined the effect of grammatically (under match vs. mismatch conditions) and group (NSs vs. L2ers), the results of which are presented in Table 7. Generally, no statistical significance was found for grammatically, although the NSs' mean reaction times were much shorter than those of L2ers.

A second ANOVA was run to examine the effects of context ([+definite, +specific], [+definite, -specific], [-definite, -specific], and [-definite, +specific]) and group (NSs vs. L2ers). As Table 8 illustrates, a main effect of group was observed in both the critical and spill-over regions, and a significant effect of context was observed in Region 1 and confirmed in Tukey tests with multiple comparisons, the results of which show that those differences were statistically significant (see Table 9).

Table 7

Two-way ANOVA (grammatically and group) for Regions 1 and 2

Region 1

Region 2

Factor

Df F value

Adjusted p value

Df

F value Adjusted p value

Grammatically

1 0.16

0.69

1

2.550.11

Group

1136.77

<.001***

1

41.42<.001***

Table 8

Two-way ANOVA (context and group) for Regions 1 and 2

Region 1

Region 2

Df F value

Adjusted p-value

Df

F value

Adjusted p value

Context

32.27

0.01**

3

1.14

.27

Group

1161.29

< .001

***

1

59.54

< .001***

Context x group

30.015

0.59

3

0.36

0.78

Table 9

Tukey HSD multiple comparisons of means for NSs and L2ers in Region 1

Mean

difference

Lower 95.0% CI

Upper 95.0% CI

Adjusted

p-value

[+definite, -specific] NSs and L2ers

313.68

161.32

466.03

< .001***

[+definite, +specific] NSs and L2ers

319.29

167.11

471.47

< .001***

[-definite, -specific] NSs and L2ers

313.56

161.38

465.74

< .001***

[-definite, +specific] NSs and L2ers

326.51

174.75

478.27

< .001***

The results of the ANOVA also suggest that there was no two-way interaction between group and context in Region 1, confirming the reported effects for both NSs and L2 learners. Furthermore, the SPRT results reveal significant differences in ...


Подобные документы

  • Countable and uncountable nouns. The articles a/an and the belong to a group of words called “determiners”. The problem of using and teaching articles is of great importance for many reasons. Different ways of teaching articles. Testing using articles.

    контрольная работа [25,1 K], добавлен 30.04.2009

  • Concept and features of the Middle English, stages and directions of its formation and development. Primary and Middle English consonants, the basic principles of articles and declination. Personal pronouns, verbs, syntax, semantics and dialects.

    презентация [380,6 K], добавлен 24.04.2014

  • General characteristics of the stylistic features of English articles, the main features. Analysis of problems the article in English as one of the most difficult. Meet the applications of the definite article, consideration of the main examples.

    доклад [15,8 K], добавлен 28.04.2013

  • English language: history and dialects. Specified language phenomena and their un\importance. Differences between the "varieties" of the English language and "dialects". Differences and the stylistic devices in in newspapers articles, them evaluation.

    курсовая работа [29,5 K], добавлен 27.06.2011

  • A critical knowledge of the English language is a subject worthy of the attention of all who have the genius and the opportunity to attain it. A settled orthography is of great importance, as a means of preserving the etymology and identity of words.

    курсовая работа [28,1 K], добавлен 14.02.2010

  • Article as a part of speech. Theoretical and practical aspect. The historical development of articles. Lexico-grammatical aspects of translation of the definite and indefinite articles. Realization of the contextual meanings of the indefinite article.

    дипломная работа [2,1 M], добавлен 14.11.2011

  • Traditional periodization of historical stages of progress of English language. Old and middle English, the modern period. The Vocabulary of the old English language. Old English Manuscripts, Poetry and Alphabets. Borrowings in the Old English language.

    презентация [281,2 K], добавлен 27.03.2014

  • Development of guidelines for students of the fifth year of practice teaching with the English language. Definition of reading, writing and speaking skills, socio-cultural component. Research issues in linguistics, literary and educational studies.

    методичка [433,9 K], добавлен 18.01.2012

  • Concept as a linguo-cultural phenomenon. Metaphor as a means of concept actualization, his general characteristics and classification. Semantic parameters and comparative analysis of the concept "Knowledge" metaphorization in English and Ukrainian.

    курсовая работа [505,9 K], добавлен 09.10.2020

  • The reasons of importance of studying of English. Use of English in communication. Need for knowledge of English during travel, dialogue with foreigners, at information search on the Internet. Studying English in Russia is as one of the major subjects.

    реферат [16,5 K], добавлен 29.08.2013

  • In the world there are thousands of different languages. How indeed modern English is optimum mean for intercourse of people of different nationalities. Knowledge of English is needed for the effective teaching subsequent work and improvement of our life.

    сочинение [13,7 K], добавлен 11.02.2009

  • Spread in the world of English as a native and first spoken. The origins of the English from the invading Germanic tribes in Britain in the 5th century and up today, the change in pronunciation. English-speaking countries of the world; American English.

    презентация [7,1 M], добавлен 09.03.2015

  • Recommendations about use of a text material and work with expressions. Rules of learning and a pronunciation of texts taking into account articles, prepositions and forms of verbs. The list of oral conversational topics on business English language.

    методичка [50,8 K], добавлен 15.02.2011

  • Linguistic situation in old english and middle english period. Old literature in the period of anglo-saxon ethnic extension. Changing conditions in the period of standardisation of the english language. The rise and origins of standard english.

    курсовая работа [98,8 K], добавлен 05.06.2011

  • The Origin of Black English. Development of Pidgin and Creole. Differences of Black English and Standard English, British English and British Black English. African American Vernacular English and its use in teaching process. Linguistic Aspects.

    дипломная работа [64,6 K], добавлен 02.11.2008

  • From the history of notion and definition of neologism. Neologisms as markers of culture in contemporary system of language and speech. Using of the neologisms in different spheres of human activity. Analysis of computer neologisms in modern English.

    научная работа [72,8 K], добавлен 13.08.2012

  • The historical background of the spread of English and different varieties of the language. Differences between British English and other accents and to distinguish their peculiarities. Lexical, phonological, grammar differences of the English language.

    курсовая работа [70,0 K], добавлен 26.06.2015

  • Features of the use of various forms of a verb in English language. The characteristics of construction of questions. Features of nouns using in English language. Translating texts about Problems of preservation of the environment and Brands in Russian.

    контрольная работа [20,1 K], добавлен 11.12.2009

  • Specific character of English language. Words of Australian Aboriginal origin. Colloquialisms in dictionaries and language guides. The Australian idioms, substitutions, abbreviations and comparisons. English in different fields (food and drink, sport).

    курсовая работа [62,8 K], добавлен 29.12.2011

  • Using constructions "There is/ There are". Form "to be going to" sentences, meaning. Test exercises with pronouns. The Future Indefinite Tense. Modal verbs, the articles, noun. Past Tenses, passive voice, the Sequence of Tenses, prepositions in English.

    тест [49,6 K], добавлен 10.12.2011

Работы в архивах красиво оформлены согласно требованиям ВУЗов и содержат рисунки, диаграммы, формулы и т.д.
PPT, PPTX и PDF-файлы представлены только в архивах.
Рекомендуем скачать работу.