A Corpus Investigation of English Cognition Verbsand their Effect on the Incipient Epistemization of Physical Activity Verbs

We are demonstrate the extent of inflectional and collocational specificity for verbs of cognition (think, know) and physical activity (strike, hit, go, run) and discuss implications this lexico-syntactic idiosyncracy has for cognitive linguistics.

Рубрика Иностранные языки и языкознание
Вид статья
Язык английский
Дата добавления 15.03.2021
Размер файла 319,1 K

Отправить свою хорошую работу в базу знаний просто. Используйте форму, расположенную ниже

Студенты, аспиранты, молодые ученые, использующие базу знаний в своей учебе и работе, будут вам очень благодарны.

Размещено на http://www.allbest.ru/

A Corpus Investigation of English Cognition Verbsand their Effect on the Incipient Epistemization of Physical Activity Verbs

Sally Rice1 and John Newman1,2

'University of Alberta

2Monash University, Australia

Abstract

verbs cognition physical activity lexica syntactic

In the spirit of NSM accounts that attempt to build up a language's full expressivity from a small set of lexical primitives, we have investigated the usage in English of basic verbs of ideation (think, know) and physical activity (strike, hit, go, run) as they take on new epistemic meanings and functions, all the while calcifying in their inflectional range. It is well known that certain verbs of cognition in English such as remember, forget, and think are grammaticalizing into pragmatic particles of epistemic stance and, consequently, 1st person singular (lsg) forms account for the majority of usages. Likewise, we have carried out systematic queries and hand-tagging of corpus returns and have found that many verbs and phrasal expressions, ideational or not, seem to be associated with rather narrow collocational patterning, argument structure, and inflectional marking in almost idiom-like and constructional fashion. Moreover, we find that expressions associated with lsg and 2nd person “cognizers” are, to a large extent, in complementary distribution, giving rise to fairly strong semantic differences in how I and you “ideate”. In this study, we demonstrate the extent of inflectional and collocational specificity for verbs of cognition and physical activity and discuss implications this lexico-syntactic idiosyncracy has for cognitive linguistics.

Keywords: cognition, corpus methods, collocation, lexico-syntax, inflectional categories, epistemization

Корпусное исследование английских глаголов мышления и их влияния на начальную эпистемизацию глаголов физического действия

Сэлли Райс1 и Джон Ньюман1,2

'Университет Альберты, Канада

2Университет Монаш, Австралия

Аннотация

С позиций подхода ЕСМ, который стремится представить всю экспрессивность языка с помощью небольшого набора лексических примитивов, мы исследовали употребление английских базовых глаголов мышления (think, know) и глаголов физического действия (strike, hit, go, run), которые принимают новые эпистемические значения и функции. Хорошо известно, что определенные глаголы мышления в английском языке, такие как remember, forget, think, могут использоваться в роли прагматических частиц эпистемического содержания и, как следствие этого, форма 1 лица единственного числа является наиболее употребимой. В результате проведения систематических исследований ручной аннотации корпуса мы обнаружили, что для многих глаголов, как и фразовых выражений, характерна достаточно узкая сочетаемость, аргументативная структура и некоторые черты идиоматичности. Более того, нами обнаружено, что в выражениях, где глаголы мышления употребляются с первым и вторым лицом, в значительной степени в дополнительной дистрибуции, наблюдаются достаточно сильные семантические различия. В данной работе мы демонстрируем специфичность коллокаций глаголов мышления и глаголов физического действия и обсуждаем значение лексико-синтаксической идиосинкразии для когнитивной лингвистики.

Ключевые слова: мышление, корпусные методы, коллокация, лексико-синтаксический анализ, инфлекционные категории, эпистемизация

INTRODUCTION

In the present study, we describe a number of relatively low-level patterns associated with basic verbs of ideation (think, know), along with other peculiar inflectional patterns in a miscellany of constructions We use small caps (e.g. THINK) for any lemma (and for referring to lemma equivalents across languages) and italics (e.g. think, thinks, thought) for all inflected forms.. In the case of the ideation verbs, it is the specific combination of subject pronouns and these verbs that will be our focus. Our interest lies in identifying recurring patterns of usage and, where possible, seeking motivation for such patterns in human experiential realities. This approach to the study of language, grounding language phenomena in broader cognitive realities, is rightly called a cognitive linguistic approach (see Dancygier 2017a for an introduction to the field of cognitive linguistics as currently practised and Dancygier 2017b for contemporary overviews of subfields). Our adoption of a corpus-based methodology to investigate these patterns reflects, too, a widely held view within cognitive linguistics that a usage-based approach is a tool of critical importance. Indeed Dancygier (2017a: 2) remarks that “actual usage is at the core of cognitive linguistics”.

Our decision to focus on the pair {think, know} is based on a number of considerations: the relatively high frequency of such verbs in ordinary discourse; the closeness of each member of the pair to the other semantically, creating potentially interesting contrasts in the details of usage; the tendency for each of these verbs to become discourse markers. These considerations suggest that these two verbs have affinities with each other that can be profitably studied at a finer-grained level than has been done to date, giving insight into why they each take different paths in terms of semantic shift and why they manifest highly skewed and individualized agreement patterns.

More specifically, the aims of this study are (i) to identify statistically significant combinations of subject/object pronouns with select English verbal expressions using corpus-based methodologies; (ii) to identify preferences for the use of 1st person in other miscellaneous constructions, prompted by our findings from (i); and (iii) to reflect on the larger significance of our findings for the field of cognitive linguistics. We begin with some relevant background research on co-occurrence patterns of number/person and verb categories (§2). We then introduce the corpus and the statistical methods used in the study (§3), present the findings (§4), and discuss the larger significance of the findings for cognitive linguistics (§5).

BACKGROUND

The co-occurrence patterns of pronouns with certain verbs have already received attention in the linguistics literature as part of the typological interest in the Hale-Silverstein person hierarchy of 1st > 2nd > 3rd (Hale 1972, Silverstein 1976), but there has been rather less interest in patterns occurring with specific inflected forms of verbs. For English, statements about co-occurring argument types (whether it is the semantics of the arguments or how hierarchies of person or animacy play out) are usually made at the lemma level.

In the context of corpus linguistic research, Sinclair (1991:8) suggested that inflectional differences may be more important in terms of their patterning than is commonly assumed, taking the inflected word form (rather than a lemma) to be the default unit of study: “There is a good case for arguing that each distinct form is potentially a unique lexical unit, and that forms should only be conflated into lemmas when their environments show a certain amount and type of similarity.” For example, in Sinclair's illustration of this approach, adjectival forms like bloody and bloodiest are kept apart in a word count of a corpus, as are is and are. Sinclair's position has been recently restated by Knowles & Don (2004: 71): “...it has become apparent that individual members of the lemma can behave independently and develop their own meanings and collocations” See also the references to earlier studies on person and number preferences with verbs in Scheibman (2001: 61--63, 2002: 1--87).. Newman & Rice's (2006: 31) notion of an “inflectional island” is very much in the same vein as Knowles and Don's remarks, referring to syntactic/semantic properties that tend to inhere in individual inflections of a verb, rather than extending across all inflected forms of the lemma. In that paper, Newman and Rice found distinctive and intriguing patterns of PRO subjects with transitive and intransitive uses of eat and drink verbs in spoken and written registers.

Recent research into patterning at the inflectional level has yielded promising results (cf. studies exploring quite specific lexical items such as Thompson & Mulac 1991, Aijmer 1997, Karkkainnen 2003, and Van Bogaert 2011 on I think; Tao 2001, 2003 on remember and forget). Many of these studies focus on the grammaticalization of what have been termed complement-taking mental predicates into complement-less pragmatic markers. Unlike our study here, the majority of these previous analyses have not been corpus-based, although they have appealed to familiar corpus notions such as high frequency and increased collocational fixedness that do have a bearing on grammatical entrenchment or what Schoonjanns (2012) has called “particulization”. He uses this concept in the context of the German ideational verb glauben `think/believe', which has both lost its 1sg pronoun, ich, and its TAM (tense-aspect-mode) inflection and emerged as a sentence-medial modal particle, glaub, with the evidential force of `maybe' or `perhaps'. While the present study is consonant with much of that prior grammaticalization research, our purpose is to examine why such grammaticalization came about in the first place through heavily skewed inflectional preferences (for 1sg.pres and 2.pres, respectively) affecting the major ideational verbs. Our aim is not to relitigate the case for the grammaticalization of these verbs into pragmatic markers, but to show how first-person singular (I) and second-person (you) ideation are associated with different semantic values which have had huge consequences for the incipient epistemization of non-ideational verbs. Not only do different types of predicates enter into the ideational arena, but they tend towards specific inflectional and collocational preferences, as we will show through a series of corpus searches and analyses. In short, the ways that “I ideate” as opposed to “you ideate” are strongly linked to connotations of I think and you know in the first place. These two epistemic constructions are different and differentially draw non-epistemic verbs and constructions into their respective orbits or, as we describe in §5, their respective “attractor basins” (in the sense of Spivey 2008).

Another uniquely valuable contribution to our understanding of inflectional level patterning in English is Scheibman's (2001) discussion of subject types, sub-categorized in terms of person and number, with different classes of verbs. She throws light on the notion of “subjectivity”, understood as how speakers and writers use linguistic devices to express their own individual perceptions, feelings, and opinions. As in our present study, Scheibman's (2001) research focuses on the preference for certain person and number choices (1st person singular, 2nd person singular, etc.) as grammatical subjects of verb types and her verb classes include cognition (know, think, remember, figure out, etc) verbs. While her study of these larger classes (alongside other broad categories) is helpful, especially when it comes to comparing results across lexical fields, we have chosen to explore linguistic patterning at a more fine-grained level, reporting on patterns involving selected individual verbs and expressions, i.e., think vs. know, go vs. run through one's mind, etc.

It is appropriate to mention, too, relevant research in Natural Semantic Metatheory (NSM; cf. Wierzbicka 1996, Goddard 1997, Goddard & Wierzbicka 2014). While NSM does not employ the highly quantitative methods of some of the works mentioned above, it succeeds in providing insightful semantic analyses building upon a set of semantic primitives. It is not a coincidence that the verbs we have chosen to focus on are among the six mental predicates recognized in later versions of the inventory of semantic primitives in NSM, namely think, know, want, feel, see, hear (using small caps here to denote these primitives, following the practice in NSM). NSM shares the broader cognitive linguistic interest in the role of ordinary bodily realities and experiences in motivating and shaping aspects of language behavior and it is not surprising that our own approach has brought us to a set of verbs that play a key role in NSM. The discussion of I think and miscellaneous other epistemic phrases of English in Wierzbicka (2006: Chapter 7) shows a further overlap between NSM and our own focus in this study. It is of interest to note that when they occur in definitions of words, these mental predicates in NSM may sometimes appear specifically with the 1SG pronoun. So, for example, the 1SG pronoun is required as the subject of WANT in the sequence of statements “many good things are happening to me now as I want; I can do many things now as I want; this is good” as part of the explication of He was happy (Goddard and Wierzbicka 2014: 103). In other publications, too, Wierzbicka has turned attention to the different semantic content associated with different choices of number/person subjects in expressions, e.g. 1sg and 3pl (e.g. people) subject frames of to have a sense that (Wierzbicka 2010: 169--176).

METHODOLOGY

Throughout this study, we rely upon the Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA, https://corpus.byu.edu/coca/) for our English usage data. COCA is a corpus of contemporary American English (Davies 2008-) and has been tagged using the CLAWS 7 tagset. It is available to users via a web interface, which is how it was accessed for this study. The corpus consists of texts dating from 1990--2017 and is being added to each year (thus, it is a “monitor corpus”). We see spoken language as being particularly relevant in the present study, since it is in spoken language that one might expect to see a greater representation of emergent constructions. Our corpus searches will therefore be restricted to the spoken component of COCA or what we will call COCAsp. COCAsp consists of transcripts of unscripted conversation from more than 150 different TV and radio programs, making up over 118 million words at the time of writing (2018). The programs on which COCAsp is based are largely concerned with American news and current affairs, along with some idiosyncratic interview-style programs. As such, the language of COCAsp may be called naturalistic for these contexts because it is interactional, but it is not necessarily natural as far as ordinary, everyday conversation is concerned.

Regardless of grammatical case, we will refer to the six pronoun forms under investigation (1sg, 2, 3sgM, 3sgF, 1pl, 3pl) simply as the pronouns (pro) without further qualification. Both upper-case and lower-case forms of the pronouns will be included in frequency counts. The decision to exclude it relates to specific interest in verbs of sentience occurring with animate, especially human, participants, rather than with inanimate. Sequences such as [PRO + verb] will be used as the basic proxy pattern for retrieving personal subject pronouns occurring with the verb forms. A refinement of this search pattern may be used to find the [subject pro + present tense verb] sequences in declarative structures such as such She knows a lot and What you know about dinosaurs is amazing, but not Does he know anything about dinosaurs? While interrogative structures would be a viable and interesting extension of the present study, they will not be included here. In terms of “precision” (i.e., how well our returns match subject and verb combinations), our proxy search for pro as the subject of a verb is high, attributable in part to the availability of the CLAWS 7 part of speech tags on the verbs, distinguishing present tense forms (vv0, vvz) from infinitival forms (vvi). The “recall” (i.e., the extent to which our returns include all the relevant subject-verb combinations), on the other hand, is not 100%. Subjects of verbs are, of course, not restricted to the position immediately to the left of the verb, even in declarative structures; rather, they can appear some distance to the left. With pronominal subjects, there is less likelihood of intervening relative clauses than with nouns (as in He who thinks before acting is wiser), but certainly adverbials can easily intervene (as in I always think of her). Recall is clearly not ideal, but, importantly, we use the same kind of search pattern in each case and the comparison across the search results is based on the methodological decision to use the same position immediately to the left of the verb in most searches. (We make an exception in the case of certain adverbs like suddenly, discussed below in §4.2.2.)

While frequency of occurrence of patterns lies at the heart of this study, we will make use of a more sophisticated (but easily understood) statistical measure in reporting on the verbs that are the main focus, i.e., the epistemic verbs, for which we have sufficient frequencies to test statistically. The statistical measure involves a calculation of standardized residuals associated with a chisquare statistic, indicating the extent to which particular pronouns occurring as the subject/object of a verb are overused or underused. In considering the patterning of PRO as the subject of a verb, the initial step is to determine the frequencies of the combination [pro + present tense of any verb] in COCAsp By “any verb”, we mean any “lexical verb”. This category excludes forms of DO, HAVE, BE, all of which have special properties in their auxiliary verb use. The frequencies in Table 1 were obtained by a series of searches using the strings “I.[pp*] [vv0]”, “you.[pp*] [vv0]”, “she.[pp*] [vvz]”, “he.[pp*] [vvz]” etc., with the “Chart” option selected to display total frequencies for each genre (“Section”) in COCA. All corpus findings were the result of searches in COCA during February 2018.. These frequencies may be called the baseline frequencies and are shown in Table 1. From these frequencies, we can see the proportions of I, you, (s)he, etc. in the whole corpus functioning as the subject of a verb in the present tense, expressed as percentages in Table 1.

Table 1

Baseline frequencies and percentages of occurrence of subject pronouns of all lexical verbs (base or present-tense forms) in COCAsp

CLAWS Tag

PROsubj

Raw Frequency

Row Total / Column Total as Percentage

Rank Order

[vv0]

I

718,184

39%

1

[vv0]

you

599,585

33%

2

[vvz]

(s)he

145,014

8%

5

[vv0]

we

207,887

11%

3

[vv0]

they

163,885

9%

4

Total

1,834,555

100%

The frequencies of the pronouns occurring with the present tense of any verb in COCAsp are “expected” to be in the same proportions as the overall proportions in Table 1 (or, more weakly, to share the same overall rank order). That is, we start with the assumption that the proportion of some phenomenon in a sub-part of the population will be identical to that found in the whole population (the “null hypothesis”, cf. Gries 2013b: 316--319) and proceed to show how likely this assumption is given the discrepancies between the observed and expected frequencies of the skewed agreement phenomenon we are investigating. Once the expected frequencies have been calculated, it is possible to compare them with the observed frequencies and evaluate the statistical significance of the difference between them, as in a chisquare test. The standardized residuals represent a standardized value of the difference between the observed and expected frequencies for each combination of pronoun and verb implemented in R (R Development Core Team 2014), obtained by calculating the differences between observed and expected frequencies, divided by the square root of the expected frequency (Agresti 2007: 38--39). Standardized residuals with values greater than +2 or less than -2 indicate statistically significant overuse or underuse in those cells. It is also helpful to display the overuse and underuse of pronouns with verb forms graphically, as in an association plot (cf. Gries 2013a: 187--188), and we will make use of these plots in the course of our exposition We used the functions chisq.test()$std for the standardized residuals and assocplot() to create the association plots in the base package of R (R Development Core Team, 2018)..

PRONOUNS + VERB PREFERENCES

The ideational verbs, think and know

We begin our discussion with the distinct frequency profiles of inflected forms of think and know in spoken English (here, in their simple present tense forms) with different agreement patterns as measured by their co-occurrence with the different subject personal pronouns. As we will argue in §5, these distributional differences have had a concomitant effect on the recruitment of non-ideational predicates and constructions to take on epistemic meanings in the language. We queried COCAsp for all subject pronouns (except it) occurring with base or present-tense forms of think and know, using the POS (part of speech) tags, vv0, vvz, and compared those frequencies with all other lexical verbs in the spoken sub-corpus occurring with the same set of pronouns. Table 2a gives the raw (observed) frequencies for think(s) with a pronominal subject while Table 2b gives the standardized residuals when compared with the raw frequencies for all other verbs.

Table 2

(a) Observed frequencies for think [pro + think.vvO | vvz] compared to frequencies of all other lexical verbs (base or 3sg present-tense forms) in the spoken subcorpus of COCA.

(b) Standardized residuals for think frequencies compared to frequencies of all other lexical verbs

(a)

I

you

(s)he

we

they

THINK

254,252

15,855

828

6,767

5,996

other

463,932

583,730

144,186

201,120

157,889

Total

718,184

599,585

145,014

207,887

163,885

(b)

I

you

(s)he

we

they

THINK

599.0677

-334.6267

-163.4517

-163.5032

-138.5127

other

-599.0677

334.6267

163.4517

163.5032

138.5127

Figure 1 shows the association plot for the distribution of pro x think In the association plots shown here, black rectangles above a dashed line correspond to cases where the observed frequency is greater than the expected frequency; white rectangles below a dashed line correspond to cases where the observed frequency is less than the expected frequency. The area of a box in such plots is proportional to the difference in observed and expected frequencies; the width of the rectangle is proportional to the square root of the expected frequency; the height of the rectangle is proportional to the standardized residual.. For present purposes, it is the relative height of the boxes, reflecting the values of the standardized residuals, that is most relevant. The black rectangles in Figure 1 show the overuse of subject pronouns with present-tense forms of verbs in a more immediate and more striking way than by inspecting numerical tables. 1sg, while hugely overrepresented with think, is greatly underrepresented across the rest of the verbal lexicon, on average.

other

Figure 1. Association plot of pro x THINK compared to other verbs in COCAsp

The pro x think distributions compared to pro x other verbs distributions for think are but half of the story. When we look at the distributional frequencies for know, we start to get a picture of the differential behaviour and distinct epistemizational attraction to concepts of first person singular ideation versus second person ideation. Table 3 gives the raw (observed) frequencies for know(s) with a pronominal subject as well as the standardized residuals when compared with the raw frequencies for all other verbs. Figure 2 shows the association plot corresponding to the distribution of pronouns given in Table 3. Comparing Figures 1 and 2, we see how know is the converse of THINK.

Table 3

(a) Observed frequencies for know [pro + know.vv01 vvz] compared to frequencies of all other lexical verbs (base or present-tense forms) in COCAsp.

(b) Standardized residuals for know frequencies compared to frequencies of all other lexical verbs

(a)

I

you

(s)he

we

they

KNOW

42,219

320,202

16,809

5,891

2,815

other

675,965

279,383

128,205

201,996

161,070

Total

718,184

599,585

145,014

207,887

163,885

(b)

I

you

(s)he

we

they

KNOW

-406.1812

745.5418

-92.84945

-217.1432

-201.8366

other

406.1812

-745.5418

92.84945

217.1432

201.8366

Figure 2. Association plot of pro x KNOW compared to other verbs in COCAsp

Our main purpose in this section is to establish, statistically, the attraction that think and KNOW have for particular inflectional forms of the subject pronouns, especially 1sg and 2 person subjects, rather than explore the particular constructions in which these combinations occur. The syntactic, semantic, and pragmatics of the individual uses of these verbs is beyond the scope of the present study. Even so, it is of interest to note the co-occurrence, indeed the juxtaposition, of I think and you know in examples such as (1a-b). In these examples, illustrating the two possible orders I think + you know and you know +1 think, we see think used with a clause complement while you know appears as a complement-less pragmatic marker (cf. §2).

(1) a. I think, you know, everything changes in politics, but I've, I've... (SPOK: NBC_MeetPress, 2007)

b. You know, I think we will see that eventually. (SPOK: NPR_ATCW, 2006)

Although we are relying on the standardized residuals to establish the statistical significance of the overrepresentation of I think and you know in the corpus, it is still instructive to consider some relevant raw frequencies related to the use of subject pronouns and lexical verbs in the present tense in the corpus. Table 4 lists the 20 most frequent base forms ([vv0]) occurring immediately to the right of I and you, respectively. In this table, we can readily see the overall preference for verbs of cognition (think, mean, know, want, guess, remember, understand, etc.) in this construction with I. think is not just the top-ranked verb in the first column of this table, it enjoys nearly two and a half times the frequency of the second-ranked verb, mean (707,880 vs. 284,116). The fourth column lists the results for the 20 most frequent base forms co-occurring with you. One sees in these results a greater variation in the semantics than with the I-verbs, with non-cognition verbs such as GO, look, say, talk, come, find, etc. making a conspicuous appearance in the list. Here, know is far and away the most frequent verb (320,202), well ahead (at 12 times the frequency!) of the second-ranked verb, want (26,762). In other words, the particular preferences for I think and you know that we see in Figures 1 and 2 do not tell the whole story about the attraction of these verbs to 1sg and 2nd person subjects, respectively; these preferences are evident in a striking way even when all present tense verbs are considered. Nevertheless, as the two most frequent [PRO-verb.PRES] bigrams in COCAsp, we have to acknowledge that I think and you know are, individually, huge constructional magnets for other expressions. It is incumbent, then, that we come to understand the particular semantic associations and connotations that imbue I think and you know since we find equally skewed distributions (by subject, object, or possessive pronoun agreement) with non-ideational expressions that have come to have epistemic force in the language, even though they were originally verbs of perception or physical action. Happily, corpus analysis can help us do this.

Table 4

The 20 most frequent sequences of I vs. you, respectively, with present tense lexical verbs (queried as [vv0]) in COCAsp

1SG + verb.PRES

frequency

rank

2 + verb.PRES

frequency

rank

I think

707,880

1

you know

320,202

1

I mean

284,116

2

you want

26,762

2

I know

201,758

3

you get

17,431

3

I want

167,388

4

you see

16,810

4

I guess

82,616

5

you think

15,855

5

I love

68,174

6

you look

15,836

6

I believe

67,618

7

you go

14,708

7

I feel

62,274

8

you say

13,350

8

I like

54,782

9

you need

10,109

9

I see

54,498

10

you take

4,743

10

I say

53,060

11

you talk

4,631

11

I hope

49,798

12

you put

4,499

12

I remember

44,954

13

you hear

4,264

13

I get

41,936

14

you like

3,818

14

I need

39,570

15

you feel

3,808

15

I understand

30,592

16

you make

3,449

16

I wish

27,346

17

you mean

3,380

17

I go

23,902

18

you start

2,812

18

I look

23,670

19

you come

2,770

19

I suppose

22,622

20

you find

2,199

20

Miscellaneous activity verbs and constructions

Having established (i) that the basic verbs think and know have highly skewed inflectional profiles and (ii) that I think and you know are uniquely privileged uses of think and KNOW and wildly dominant inflectionally speaking, we now turn to a range of verbal constructions that, when taken literally, have nothing to do with ideation, but which clearly have undergone epistemization processes in the language. That is, certain verbal constructions are turning into expressions about ideation or knowledge validation and they are turning up with highly skewed inflectional profiles of their own.

While we find these expressions interesting as cognitive linguists because they have taken on meanings beyond the literal and the physical, they prove to be especially fascinating to us as corpus linguists because they display similar inflectional skewing as we find with the two basic cognition verbs explored in §4.1. Moreover, through an examination of their frequencies by agreement and TAM, we can gain insight into how first person singular ideation is construed in English, compared to ideation affecting second persons. If language change or semantic shift is driven in part by analogy, then a better understanding of the different semantic associations affecting verbal expressions by person helps us make the larger point advocated by Sinclair and others that the inflected lexical form is the proper starting point for lexico-syntactic analysis, not the idealized lemmatized form. In this section, because the frequency counts are relatively low, we will only report raw frequency with no further statistical analysis. It is worth noting that the cognizer in the following constructions are not encoded as the subject of the verb, but as a down-stream thematic participant, construed as a patient or as the object of a preposition. The subject is generally a pleonastic, it, or the headless relative pronoun, what.

It/What strike/hit PRO

The two physical verbs that have re-lexicalized or, actually, constructionalized into verbs of ideation the most are strike and hit. Indeed, the participial adjective, striking, collocates most frequently with nouns that are associated with epistemic realization or discernment, such as thing, resemblance, contrast, example, difference, and similarity. With strike and hit, the cognizer presents as the direct object, as in it struck me or what hit him or as the prepositional object with progressive forms of strike, as in it was striking to me; therefore, our COCAsp searches involved variants of these search strings: [what^t [strikejhit] (p*)] or [what [be] striking (to) (p*)]. Tables 5 and 6 summarize the returns for strike and hit, respectively, by TAM and construction (cognizer is a pronominal DO or X). Corpus examples follow in (2) and (3).

Table 5

Observed frequencies for [what/it strike (to) pro] in COCA^.

[*All 44 instances of what struck you are questions, with the what functioning as a bona fide question word, as opposed to the function of what to introduce a pseudo-cleft as in (2a).]

strike

me

you

him

her

us

them

Row

Total

what strikes PRO

111

26

137

what struck PRO

144

44*

1

1

4

194

what's striking PRO

1

1

what is striking to PRO

3

3

what was striking to PRO

6

6

it strikes PRO

191

3

2

3

199

it struck PRO

105

3

2

2

6

3

121

it is striking to PRO

3

3

it was striking to PRO

2

2

Column Total

566

76

5

3

13

3

666

Column Total/Row Total as Percentage

85%

11%

< 1%

< 1/2%

2%

< /%

100%

(2) a. He can be personable, but he also can be very serious. Now what is striking to me is that he still seems removed from who he's working for. I mean, he was clearly referencing Harvey Weinstein in respect for women, or that would be the best guess (SPOK: CNN_Anderson Cooper, 2017).

b. It's odd that he--well, it strikes me as a little bit odd that he continually talks about his struggle to get there, the reasons behind it, struggle, it's well established, it's done. I have a feeling he should stop talking like that if there's any possibility that he's going to get in this thing... (SPOK: CBS_FaceTheNation, 2015).

c. Before this kind of gradual, almost indistinguishable, process of- of corruption. You know, it struck me again, in- in- in going back through it, how much it really is a parable of the dark side of the moon of the democratic proposition... (SPOK: NPR_Weekend, 1996).

Table 6

Observed frequencies for [what/it hit pro] in COCAsp. Note the pseudo-cleft uses of what in (3a-b). The relatively high values for it hit/hits you shouldn't be taken at face value.

Nearly all of them are used generically or refer back to the 1sg speaker, as in (3d)

hit

me

you

him

her

us

them

Row Total

what hits PRO

2

1

1

4

what hit PRO

9

7

12

1

3

8

40

what's hitting PRO

1

1

it hits PRO

13

20

3

1

4

41

it hit PRO

79

16

4

6

4

2

111

it's hitting PRO

1

2

1

1

5

it is hitting PRO

1

1

Column Total

105

44

21

7

9

17

203

Column Total / Row

Total as Percentage

52%

22%

10%

3.5%

4.5%

8%

100%

(3) a. Well, I think that-what hits me about this ethics stuff, Robert, is that I'm surprised that the Democrats don't see an opening with campaign finance reform. (SPOK: NPR_ATC, 1995)

b. One of the lawyers for detainees approached me and said, I want my clients' art to be exhibited. I said, what do you mean? There's art made at Guantanamo. What hit me at first was how normal they seem. Shouldn't their drawings be so much more angry? (SPOK: PBS_Newshour, 2017)

c. Every once in a while it hits me. I'll be driving along or whatever, just by myself, and start to think about it, and it was really close. I mean any -- a foot either way, an inch either way, it would have been over for me. (SPOK: ABC_Primetime, 1994)

d. You know, after the firefight 's over and the adrenaline rush is over and you started -- you know, you're all soaking wet and just feel like your legs won't hold you, you know, it hits you. I just took a life. (SPOK: Dateline_NBC, 2008)

It is apparent from the counts, the examples, and the brief commentary in this section that strike and hit, when used to convey mental (not physical) force, have an overwhelming preference for 1sg objects construed as the target of sudden realization. More than three-quarters of the returns in COCAsp for this family of [what/it strike/hit PRO] constructions are about 1sg ideation. Obviously, strike and hit bring many semantic associations from the physical world when used figuratively. They both suggest punctual, telic, and dynamic action, which, we argue, carries over into how 1SG ideation is construed more generally. We return to this point in §5.

It dawn on PRO

For it to dawn on someone is a particularly nice figurative expression in English to describe epistemic realization. The various TAM-inflected forms of what is otherwise a concrete verb, dawn, describing the path of the sun and the return of daylight (widely associated with consciousness and understanding), show an overwhelming preference for 1sg prepositional objects, the nominal relation that encodes the cognizer in this construction. Table 7 shows the raw frequencies from COCAsp by TAM and person of the prepositional object. We have broadened the searches to also include the adverbs which collocate with it dawns/dawned on. We provide some actual corpus returns in (4).

Table 7

Observed frequencies for [it (adv) dawn on pro] in COCA^

dawn on

me

you

him

her

us

them

Row Total

it dawns on PRO

3

1

1

5

it dawned on PRO

33

3

9

1

1

2

49

it never dawned on PRO

9

2

11

it just dawned on PRO

8

1

9

it really dawned on PRO

8

1

9

it finally dawned on PRO

4

3

1

8

it suddenly dawned on PRO

6

1

7

it slowly dawned on PRO

1

2

3

it gradually dawned on PRO

1

1

2

it fully dawned on PRO

1

1

it probably dawned on PRO

1

1

Column Total

74

4

13

3

7

4

105

Column Total / Row Total as Percentage

71%

4%

12%

3%

6%

4%

100%

(4) a. I was meeting people and talking to them, and while talking to them, it dawned on me -- oh, my God, Nick, you're talking about something I've heard of. I know this song. (SPOK: NPR_Fresh Air, 2015)

b. I don't know how we were lucky enough to figure that out, but -- and it suddenly dawned on me what I truly have in common with everybody else is this one man's vision affected all of us. (SPOK: NPR_Sunday, 2000)

A brief final point about the [it DAWN on PRO] construction and the collocating adverbs listed in Table 7. For the most part, the rather absolute and categorical never, just, finally, and fully, along with the intensified really and suddenly show a marked preference for 1sg, as does the construction as a whole. We do not regard it as incidental that the less forceful or dynamic adverbs slowly and gradually, or the indeterminate probably, show a slight preference for non-1sg cognizers. A point we make in §5 is that a range of somewhat covert semantic notions like these seem to be attached to the way 1st person singular ideation is construed. These are not necessarily associations evident in I think, but which nevertheless guide the non-ideational expressions that come to take on epistemic force towards or away from 1sg.

Vstasis in/on PRO's mind

A thought, idea, or bit of knowledge can be in or on one's mind in English. Such expressions suggest simple, stative locative constructions, far from the dynamic construals afforded by the likes of it struck someone, it hit someone, or it dawned on someone examined previously. Nevertheless, the fact that these expressions are based on a spatial metaphor and metonymy [viz. the place (mind) is the locus of activity (thinking) happening in that place]. While 1SG cognizers were prevalent in those other constructions, the more static, in/on one's mind, show only modest preferences for 1st person. Table 8 presents the returns from COCAsp for in PRO's mind and on PRO's mind, respectively. A handful of actual returns from the corpus follow in (5).

Fewer than half of the examples in COCAsp of the [in/on PRO mind] construction involve a 1sg cognizer (in the form of the possessor of mind). Indeed, these constructions seem to be better distributed across all the potential sentient players: 1sg (47%), 2 (30%), 3sgm/f (17%), 1pl (3%), 3pl (3%), in proportions far closer to those “background” frequency distributions reported in Table 1 for all lexical verbs, as represented in COCAsp. The rank order is nearly the same, for example: 1SG (#1), 2 (#2), and then the rest at a distance. The lack of overwhelming attraction to 1SG suggests that the semantic properties associated with the fairly stative and locative [in/on PRO mind] construction are fairly neutral, person-wise. As we'll see below, the more active and dynamic the figurative expression, the more it displays an attraction to a 1sg cognizer.

Table 8

Observed frequencies for [in | on [app*] mind] in COCAsp

my

your

his

her

our

their

Row Total

in PRO's mind

1,915

1,105

384

128

56

134

3,722

on PRO's mind

205

235

189

55

74

19

777

Column Total

2,120

1,340

573

183

130

153

4,499

Column Total / Row Total as Percentage

47%

30%

13%

4%

3%

3%

100%

(5) a. I mean -- I thought and, you know, knowing how I felt about him. I was angry, because in my mind he was doing that to--that was like his parting gift, right? (SPOK: CNN_The Lead with Jake Tapper, 2017)

b. You know, I found that some of them never even pulled a gun out. They shoot -- you know, they just reached down and grabbed the gun and twisted their holster and fired right through the holster. So in your mind, you think because we've always shown Westerns that they take it out and shoot -- some of them never took them out. (SPOK: NPR_Fresh Air, 2016)

c. you don't wake up in the morning and immediately start thinking about that. What's on people's minds is what's on your mind and my mind and everybody else's mind, and that is how am I going to provide for my family? (SPOK: CBS_ThisMorning, 2012)

d. That's why it's weighing very heavy. It's been weighing heavy for 37 years on his mind. I think he really wants to tell it. (SPOK: NBC_Dateline, 2005)

Vmotion through PRO's mind

Similar to the in/on one's mind expressions just examined, an idea, thought, or realization can pass through one's mind, in a slightly more dynamic fashion. Because motion verbs are involved, we have naturally categorized these as activity expressions. Admittedly, the epistemic or ideational sense is brought about figuratively by the presence of the locative nominal, mind. However, the choice of verb is somewhat affected by the choice of possessive pronoun in ways reminiscent of the 1sg vs. 2 person differences noted above in other expressions. Far and away, the most frequent verb to enter into this construction is go, a nearly manner-less verb of motion. The more force-dynamic the verb, however, the more likely it is being used to express ideation in my mind/head. Table 9 gives the lemmatized frequencies for ideation constructed with verbs of motion through the mind or head. Some actual returns from COCAsp are presented in (6).

Table 9

Observed frequencies for [vv0/vvz through [app*] mind] in COCA^. Here, we only present lemmatized results because we are specifically focusing on choice of verb with different prepositional objects, not TAM forms. The verbs are impressionistically arranged in order of increasing degree of energy, punctualness, or forcefulness, rather than frequency.

When the locus of motion is head, not mind, the counts are given
in parentheses. Aggregate counts are given in the row marked Total
a. Give me more of what went through your mind as you read this thing, that made you say, "I have to have it”. (SPOK: CBS_SundayMorning, 1993)

b. A...


Подобные документы

  • The process of translation, its main stages. Measuring success in translation, its principles. Importance of adequacy in translation, cognitive basis and linguistics. Aspects of cognition. Historical article and metaphors, especially their transfer.

    курсовая работа [48,6 K], добавлен 24.03.2013

  • Рractical and theoretical value of the types of Phrasal verbs, the structure and their role in the English Grammar. Defining, analyze and classification of Phrasal verbs. List of Phrasal verbs. Meanings of phrasal verbs with different prepositions.

    курсовая работа [32,7 K], добавлен 17.01.2011

  • Modal verbs in middle English. Functions of modal verbs in modern English. The meaning of modal verbs in translation. Differences and peculiarities of the usage of modal verbs in newspapers and fiction. The usage of modal verbs in business English.

    курсовая работа [59,7 K], добавлен 27.09.2012

  • The definition of the verb. The function of Phrasal verbs. The structure and meaning of Phrasal verbs. Classification of Phrasal verbs. Preposition and postposition. Verbs with preposition and noun. Verbs with postposition. English Phrasal Verbs Lists.

    курсовая работа [32,5 K], добавлен 17.01.2011

  • The rules and examples of using modal verbs in English: may, mights, can, could, allow. The difference of meaning between verbs. Using perfect infinitive to express an unfulfilled obligation. Examples of Absence of obligation and unnecessary action.

    презентация [20,7 K], добавлен 29.09.2011

  • А complex comparison of morphological characteristics of English and Ukrainian verbs. Typological characteristics, classes and morphological categories of the English and Ukrainian verbs. The categories of person and number, tenses, aspect, voice, mood.

    дипломная работа [162,2 K], добавлен 05.07.2011

  • Translating of suggestion into the English language. Use of regular shape of participle. The use of correct times of verbs is in suggestion. Putting of verbs in brackets in Gerund or Infinitive. Development of skills of business intercourse in English.

    контрольная работа [27,1 K], добавлен 04.03.2011

  • Degrees of comparison of adjectives and adverbs, тhe generala word order in the English offer. Impersonal and indefinite-personal offers. Correct and irregular verbs. Modal verbs and their substitutes. Concord of tenses in the main and additional offers.

    учебное пособие [208,0 K], добавлен 26.10.2009

  • Constituent analyses of the sentence. Complication of predicate and types of complications. The link-verbs in English and their translation into Uzbek and Russian. Transitivity of verbs and the problems of translating them into Uzbek, Russian languages.

    дипломная работа [295,6 K], добавлен 21.07.2009

  • Can, may, We can compare may and can. Must. Must and May compared. To have to. To be to. Must, to have to and to be to compared. Ought to. Shall and should. Must, Should and ought to compared. Should + Perfect infinitive.

    дипломная работа [35,2 K], добавлен 22.07.2006

  • The category of activity and passivity. Basic Grammatical categories. Peculiarities of using sentences with the verb in the passive voice. Ways of expressing the passive voice. The passive constructions. The implicit agent in English passives.

    курсовая работа [40,7 K], добавлен 15.03.2014

  • The roles of the student, the teacher and the language researcher in understanding the motivation to learn another language. The importance of teaching phrasal verbs and prepositions. Guessing and explaining meanings of phrasal verbs "come" and "go".

    дипломная работа [82,4 K], добавлен 10.09.2013

  • The development of Word Order. Types of syntactical relations words in the phrase, their development. The development of the composite sentence. The syntactic structure of English. New scope of syntactic distinctions and of new means of expressing them.

    лекция [22,3 K], добавлен 02.09.2011

  • Categorization is a central topic in cognitive psychology, in linguistics, and in philosophy, precisely. Practical examples of conceptualization and categorization in English, research directions of these categories in linguistics at the present stage.

    презентация [573,5 K], добавлен 29.05.2015

  • When we have time for leisure, we usually need something that can amuse and entertain us. Some people find that collecting stamps, badges, model cars, planes or ships, bottles, or antiques are relaxing hobbies. Free time is organized in many schools.

    сочинение [5,2 K], добавлен 04.02.2009

  • Concept and features of the Middle English, stages and directions of its formation and development. Primary and Middle English consonants, the basic principles of articles and declination. Personal pronouns, verbs, syntax, semantics and dialects.

    презентация [380,6 K], добавлен 24.04.2014

  • What is Climate. Science is the search for knowledge. Records changes of Climate for period million years. The activity Modern Climate Systems. What is the Greenhouse Effect. The past and current trends in climate change. The way to solve the problem.

    презентация [8,3 M], добавлен 21.02.2011

  • Theoretical bases of the economic and legal substantiation of realization of innovative activity. The technique of the estimation of the innovative project in public health services. Personnel management in scientific organizations, and life safety.

    дипломная работа [70,4 K], добавлен 21.06.2010

  • Filling in the blanks with a correct form of the phrasal verb to put. Saying the same in a different way. Phrasal verbs "to get", "to look", "to take". Expressing the idea using the phrasal verb. Difficulties in progress of learning foreign languages.

    презентация [1,2 M], добавлен 19.04.2014

  • The subject of the sentence in two grammatical categories: number and person. Grammatical categories of the verbals. Morphological classification of verbs. The main difference between the strong and weak verbs. The principal forms and minor groups.

    презентация [200,7 K], добавлен 20.10.2013

Работы в архивах красиво оформлены согласно требованиям ВУЗов и содержат рисунки, диаграммы, формулы и т.д.
PPT, PPTX и PDF-файлы представлены только в архивах.
Рекомендуем скачать работу.