Revisiting the piagetian test of conservation of quantities of liquid: argumentation within the adult child interaction
Orientation to concrete actions and achievements of argumentation theories - a specific feature of he neo piagetian and neo vygotskian approaches of collaborative interactions. Components of the piagetian test of conversation of quantities of liquid.
Рубрика | Психология |
Вид | статья |
Язык | английский |
Дата добавления | 11.07.2018 |
Размер файла | 25,0 K |
Отправить свою хорошую работу в базу знаний просто. Используйте форму, расположенную ниже
Студенты, аспиранты, молодые ученые, использующие базу знаний в своей учебе и работе, будут вам очень благодарны.
Размещено на http://www.allbest.ru
Размещено на http://www.allbest.ru
The neo piagetian and neo vygotskian approaches of collaborative interactions within goaldirected activities and the recent advances of argumentation theories call the researchers' attention to alternative modes of considering argumentation as a process within an inter action and not only as a result. In this paper we suggest a way to revisit the classical piagetian test of conservation of quantities of liquid, in order to explore to what extent children's capacity to provide the arguments expected by Piaget is in fact coconstructed within the adult-child interaction. For Piaget, when children are able to back up their conservation judgements with logical arguments, it is a sign that they have reached the stage of concrete operations. Our hypothesis is that such logical arguments are also the fruit of a coconstruction during a conversation in which both interlocutors are responsible for the outcome.
Piaget and logic.
Since the 1920s, when Piaget spent time in Paris with Binet trying to assess children's intelligence, the observation of children's arguments has become an important element in his method of interview (Piaget, 1924, 1926; Piaget and Inhelder, 1966). Piaget suggest ed that confronting children with different points of view in various situations was more relevant that simply asking them to answer questions, in order to have access to their modes of reasoning. Piaget described thereafter his method of «critical» or «clinical» interview as engaging in conversations with children, and granting special importance to «countersuggestions» as invitations to defend their answers. This should allow the psychologist to assess the structure of the child's reasoning and not only the individual responses. Piaget described the growth of thinking as organized by logical structures that are gradually modified, during the course of the development, to become more and more powerful and integrative. This is supposed to happen through an autoequilibration process that becomes active when the children encounter failures or contradictions and try to overcome them. Piaget has viewed formal logical reasoning as defining the structural endpoint of cognitive development. We are particularly interested in Smedslund's (1970, 1977) questioning of the relation between logic and reasoning during a task, in which he pointed to the distinction between the experimental set ting as viewed by adults and children, showing how it is crucial to consider the interpretation of the task in order to understand the situation and to assess whether people reason logically.
Our claim is that Piaget's exclusive attention to the logical structures of the child's thought, and to the child's statements as a sign of them, led Piaget into an underestimation of the social and conversational dynamics involved in his elicitation of arguments from the child. Even if some of his theoretical writings can be understood as a call to study the cognitive dynamics resulting from cooperation between people with different points of views, Piaget has seldom studied empirically this point. The study of argumentation in its con text remains to be done. For this reason, we assume that there is a need to revisit his classical study, and in particular his prototypical test on conservation of quantities of liquid, with the possibility to reconsider the argumentation in children's talk beyond Piaget's own reductionistic logicism on these matters.
Argumentation has been the object of research during the last decades (e. g. Kuhn, 1991; Johnson and Johnson, 1994; Golder and Schneuwly, 1996; Schwarz, Neuman and Biezuner, 2000; Erduran, Osborne and Simon, 2004; Muller Mirza and Perret Clermont, 2009; Rigotti and Greco Morasso, 2009). Their findings con tribute to the understanding of the role of argumentation as a specific form of dialogical social interaction. Research is needed to better understand why the activity of argumentation is often limited and constrained to very poor forms and to study the process by which children and adults acquire argumentative skills. How can this learning be supported?
The advances in argumentation theories (van Eemeren and Grootendorst, 2004; Rigotti, 2006; Greco Morasso, 2008) propose different ways to understand argumentation as a pragmatic process. Arguments are constructed and considered not only in relation with other arguments and not as isolated elements of a discourse, but as nested in communication processes with their implicit, their goals, and their «manoeuvring».
In this perspective, we will not to look exclusively at the quality of arguments during an interaction between children or between children and adults, but consider argumentation as a collective construction of the discourse, constrained by the dimensions of the communicative context in which it is produced (Duranti and Goodwin, 1992; Perret-Clermont, 2006; Rigotti and Rocci, 2006).
A second motive to reconsider the investigation of argumentation in piagetian tests is that we make the hypothesis that, contrarily to his intention, Piaget in fact did not really study children's argumentations, but the result of very specific types of conversations («clinical interviews») between the experimenter and the child around a task. Over the past decades, different studies have already been devoted to children's under standing of piagetian questions when embedded in different narratives (Donaldson, 1978; Light, 1986; Light, Gorsuch and Newmann, 1987; Light and Perret Clermont, 1989), and to the influence of social factors in symmetrical and asymmetrical power relations (Krstic and Baucal, 2003; Psaltis, Duveen, 2006). Other studies have demonstrated how the architecture of intersubjectivity structures the meanings deployed in the conversation (Rommetveit, 1976), how the partners scaffold can lead to different understandings (Schwarz, Perret Clermont, Trognon and Marro, 2008), and how the competence that a person can demonstrate is affected by the relationship context (Schubauer-Leoni, 1986; Grossen, 1988; Nicolet, 1995). Marro Clement, Trognon and Perret-Clermont (1999) have also described the interactions between children discussing around the notion of conservation of quantities of liquid when they don't share the same point of view. The results of these lines of study have demonstrated that even a supposedly similar context may turn out not to be the same for each participant. This is made clear by the minute observation of what happens when children are asked to solve a task (Muller Mirza, Baucal, Perret-Clermont and Marro, 2003; Tartas, Perret-Clermont, 2008). Another relevant aspect concerns the repeated question effect in the conservation test, specifically studied by Baucal and Stepanovic (2006), following the idea that children expected «one would never ask the identical question twice if a significant change had not occurred» (Rose and Blank, 1974, p. 499). These studies have demonstrated that the issue of the repeated question and its role is still open: in particular, we need to consider in detail the relevance of the talk as talk in context. Within similar lines of research, another series of studies have showed, by the observation of the everyday activities in different contexts of socialization (such as classrooms and families), how competencies, reasoning and argumentation are imbedded in talk and, more generally, in social situations (Pontecorvo, 1987; 2004; Pontecorvo and Arcidiacono, 2007).
Therefore, as talk is far from being always argumentative, we think that it is important to investigate specifically how and under which circumstances the argumentation between the experimenter and the child is coconstructed within their interaction and when it is likely to be argumentative. In the following part of the paper, we will present the specific situation of the piagetian test of conservation of quantities of liquid, in order to introduce a revisiting study.
The piagetian test of conversation of quantities of liquid.
The conservation of quantities of liquid is one of the most famous piagetian tests for assessing concrete operations in children (typically 5 to 7 years old). From a psychological point of view, Piaget has considered the need for conservation as a kind of functional a priori of thought. Piaget and Szeminska (1941) have studied the construction of the notion of conservation via a series of experiments with continuous quantities: the test of conservation of quantities of liquid was part of this work. Typically, it concerned a situation in which a child was given two cylindrical glasses of equal dimensions (A and A') containing the same quantity of liquid. The content of A was then poured into two smaller containers of equal dimensions (B and B') and the child was asked whether the quantity of liquid poured from A into (B + + B') was still equal to that in A'. Then, the liquid in B could then be poured again into two smaller, equal containers (C and C'), and the liquid in B' poured into two other containers C'' and C''' identical with C and C'. Questions as to the equality between (C + C') and B', or between (C + C' + C'' + C''') and A' were then asked. In this way, the quantities of liquid were subdivided in a variety of ways, and each time the problem of conservation was put in the form of a question as to equality or nonequality of the quantities with one of the original containers. Eventually, the experimenter would take another glass (D, taller and thinner), and pour the liquid into D from glass B and B'. The child was asked to compare the quantities present in glasses B + B' and D: «Is there more liquid in one glass or another or is there the same amount in both glasses? Why?». Piaget invited the child to react to countersuggestions in order to solicit argumentation, with the intention to understand the structure of the child's thought. For Piaget, the answers of the child were the symptoms of his operational stage. We quote below an excerpt of a piagetian interview with a child (Piaget and Szeminska, 1941) in its «canonical» form.
Excerpt 1.
Source: Piaget and Szeminska [23, p. 20]. Participants: experimenter (exp.), child (Clairette, female, age: 4,0 years old).
Table 1
1. Exp.: tu as une amie? |
Have you got a friend? |
|
2. Clairette: Oui, Odette. |
Yes, Odette. |
|
3. Exp.: Eh bien, tu vois on |
Well look, we're giving you, |
|
te donne a toi, Clairette,` un verre de sirop rouge ((A rempli aux 3/4)) et a` Odette un verre de sirop bleu ((A', meme niveau)). Estce qu'une de vous a plus a boire que l'autre?` 4. Clairette: La meme chose. |
Clairette, a glass of red juice ((A, 3/4 full)), and we're giv ing Odette a glass of blue juice ((A', also 3/4 full)). Has one of you more to drink than the other? The same. |
Exp.: Voila ce que Clairette This is what Clairette does: fait: elle verse son sirop dans she pours her juice into two deux autres verres ((B et B', other glasses ((B and B', ainsi rempli jusqu' a mi` which are thus half filled)). hauteur)). Estce que Clairet Has Clairette the same as te a la meme chose qu'Odette? Odette?
Clairette: Odette a plus. Odette has more.
Exp.: Pourquoi? Why?
Clairette: Parce qu'on a mis Because less has been put
Table 2
moins ((elle montre le niveau |
((she points to the levels in |
|
en B et B')). |
B and B')). |
|
9. ((on verse le sirop d'Odette |
((Odette's juice is poured into |
|
en C et C')). |
C and C')) |
|
Clairette: c'est la meme chose ((exp transvase le sirop de B et B' en D, plus mince et еtroit` )). Exp.: Et maintenant? Clairette: C'est moi quia plus Exp.: Pourquoi? |
It's the same ((exp pours juice from B and B' into D, taller and thinner)). And now? I've got more. Why? |
Clairette: On a verse dans It is poured into that glass` ce verre ((D, montre le n ((pointing to the level in D)), veau)) et ici ((C et C')) pas. and here ((C and C')) not.
Exp.: Mais avant c'etait la But before was it the same?` meme chose?
Table 3
16. Clairette: Oui. |
Yes. |
|
17. Exp.: Et maintenant? |
And now? |
|
18. Clairette: C'est moi qui a |
I've got more ((Clairette's |
|
plus ((ensuite on reverse le |
juice is then poured back from |
|
sirop de Clairette de D dans B et B')). |
D into B and B')). |
|
19. Exp.: Tu vois, Clairette |
Look, Clairette pours hers like |
|
verse aussi comme Odette. |
Odette. Then, the blue juice |
|
Alors tout le sirop bleu ense |
all together ((C + C')) and |
mble ((C et C')) et tout le the red juice all together ((B + rouge ensemble ((B et B')), + B')) it is the same?
estce que c'est la meme chose?
Table 4
25. Exp.: Qu'est ce qu'il faut |
What must we do so that |
|
faire pour qu'Odette ait la |
Odette has the same to drink? |
|
meme chose a boire? Clairette: Il faut prendrece petit verre ((C''', dans lequel elle verse une partie du sirop de C)). Exp.: Mais c'est la meme chose a boire, ou une a plus` que l'autre? Clairette: Odette a plusa boire. Exp.: Pourquoi? Clairette: Parce qu'elle atrois verres ((C presque vide, C' et C''', tandis que Clairette a C'' plein et B')). |
To take that little glass ((C''', into which she pours part of C)). But is it the same to drink, or has one more than the other one? Odette has more to drink. Why? Because she has three glasses ((C almost empty, C' and C''', while Clairette has C'' full and B')). |
By interviewing children doing these pourings, Piaget demonstrated that quantities do not remain constant in children's minds. He describes three developmental phaes: absence of conservation (perceptional aspects mislead the child), intermediate stage (the child starts operating mentally but still oscillates between perceptional centra tions and coordinations), and necessary conservation (it has become obvious by the child that quantities are con served according to their main arguments: identity of the juice, e. g. «nothing taken away, nothing added», compensations of the different dimensions of the glass, e. g. «thinner but wider», and reversibility, e. g. «if you pour it back you can see that nothing has changed»). Children are moving from a first step in which they only consider uncoordinated perceptions to a later stage in which a process of logical (operatory) coordination is established.
Methodological aspects.
In the following part of the paper we present an exploratory study that revisits the piagetian test of conservation of quantities of liquid. Our main interest is to explore, in a qualitative way, how conversations between the adult and the child are coconstructed within this specific setting and if the child is really given an opportunity to argue.
To some extend, we consider the test of the conservation of quantities of liquid as prototypical of piagetian situations in which a child is called to engage in conversation with a psychologist to answer questions and to provide explanations.
Aims and hypothesis.
The goal of this study is to analyze specifically the conversational strategies of the interactants, in order to explore to what extent the children's answers are co constructed within the interaction with the adult and not only a sign of the child's competences. Our hypothesis is that the child's capacity to argue is not only a sign of concrete operations properly mastered to back up conservation judgements, but also a sign of the conversational competence of both the adult and the child.
Procedure and instruments.
We have designed an experimental procedure to administer the test of conservation of quantities of liquid to children individually. The scope was to follow the traditional piagetian procedure: an adult tests a child's understanding of the notion of conservation of quantities via a conversation about the effects of pouring juice into glasses of different shapes. The experimenter and the child were seated at the same table. At the beginning, two identical glasses (A and A') were filled to the same level and the child was asked whether they each contained the same amount. Once the child had established that it was the case (sometimes after having added a few additional drops), the content of one (glass A') was poured into another (taller and thinner) glass (B). The child was then asked whether the two glasses (A and B) still contained the same quantity of liquid. Then, the content of B was poured back into A' and the child was asked the same question concerning A and A'. When the child had established again the equality of the initial quantities A and A', the content of A was poured into another (smaller and larger) glass (C), and the child was asked again to discuss the relative quantities in A' and C.
Participants and data collection/
We have investigated 28 children aged from 5 to 7 years old; we trained two adults (students in psychology and education) on the experimental procedure. We videotaped all the interactions between students and children in the same room of the children's school. All the children were living in the French-speaking part of Switzerland, in a small village of the Neuchatel region.
Qualitative analysis of the data.
In this paper we do not present all aspects of our research. We want to consider some cases in which children's conversational moves appear closely linked to the adult's explicit or implicit suggestions and cannot be attributed to the child own selfgoverned logical thinking. We intend to explore how the students we trained have managed their role, to what extend they were diverged from the piagetian script2 prescribed, and what the children have produced as a result of the conversation with this specific adult. We will present and discuss three excerpts of videotaped conversations (for the simple form of transcription we used, see Appendix 1; for the original French transcription, see Appendix 2). A child and two adults were present (one directly inter acting with the child, and another managing the video camera). For all participants, fictitious names replace real names in order to ensure anonymity in the presentation and in the analysis of the excerpts.
Adult's suggestions and children's statements.
In this section we observe a situation in which the child is asked to answer the same question several times during the test. We consider the following excerpt as a sign of how children can show their competences at a conversational level, even if it is not necessarily a sign of their cognitive level in a piagetian sense.
Excerpt 2.
Code of videorecording: Clpr1. Pretest 1st grade. Participants: experimenter (Joseph), child (Manon, female; age: 7,3 years old) ((the child has established the equality of the quantities of liq uid in glasses A and A'. Then, the adult changes the set of glass es, using A' and B))
Exp.: So there. now, let's take another glass, pour allyour juice in this glass ((exp pours the juice from the glass A to the glass B)) then, if now I drink from my glass and you drink from your glass, will we have as much ((in the two glasses A' and B)) or will someone have more juice, will someone have less juice?
Manon: As much.
Exp: As much. can you tell me why we are having asmuch?
Manon: Because that one is thinner ((glass B)).
Exp.: Thinner.
Manon: This one is larger ((glass A')).
Exp.: And then, we will hence have the same thing.
Manon: Ya ya.
Exp.: Even if one is thinner?
Manon: Yeah.
((the child has established the equality of the quantities of liquid in glasses A and A'. Then, the adult changes the set of glass es, using A and C)).
Exp.: I pour all the juice in this glass now ((from the glass A to the glass C)) and now, if I drink from my glass and you drink from your glass, we will have as much or will someone have more juice, will someone have less juice?
Manon: As much.
Exp.: Always as much, now you continue to explain tome why there we will have as much?
Manon: Because that one is thinner ((glass A)) yeah and ((the juice)) then it goes further up because it is thinner, that one ((glass C)) is more widened, ((the juice)) it does that it gives less. it is larger. yeah.
Exp.: Ok, perfect! not more than that, we have finished, I thank you very much.
In this excerpt, the child answers the first question (turn 7) about the quantities of juice in two different glasses, declaring that the amounts are the same in the containers (turn 8). She explains to the experimenter why there is the same quantity of juice, by referring to the shapes of the glasses (turn 10 «this is thinner», and turn 12 «this one is larger»), even if she does not make explicit that one compensates for the other. In turn 13, it is the experimenter who draws the conclusion assuming (rightly or wrongly) that it was implicit in the child's answer («and so, we will have the same»). The piagetian script would have instead required from him to test the child's answer by asking her to justify it fully or by making a countersuggestion: the answer alone has no value. It is the argumentation given by the child that allows to understand how she reasons.
When the experimenter, later in the sequence and using another set of glasses (A and C), is asking again a question to the child, we observe that Manon provides the answers she had provided the first time, as if she was following the previous script. In fact, turns 22 and 24 are similar to the turns 8, 10 and 12: the child repeats that there is the same quantity of juice because of the shapes of glasses. Even the experimenter is following the same conversational strategy used in the first part of the sequence: in turn 23 he asks the child to justify again why there is the same quantity in the two glasses (instead of opening a discussion about it). The invitation to «continue» can be understood by the child as a suggestion to follow with the same answers as those that seemed to been successful during the first part of the interaction. However, the script of the piagetian inter view with a child does not require at all this suggestion; on the contrary, it is supposed to open possibilities for the child to decentrate from her first perspective. Here, Manon is referring to each glass as thinner and larger (turn 24), and the experimenter ends the sequence (turn 25) even validating (although in sign of thanks) as «perfect» the answer of the child.
As a result, in this excerpt the child who is interacting with the adult maintains the same responses along the sequence. This behaviour seems to have been induced by the adult. As a consequence it seems that the interventions of the child are not (or not exclusively) signs of her reasoning, but rather signs of her capacity to adapt to the conversational demands of the adult.
The adult's diversions from the script and the influence to the child In the next excerpt we present a situation in which there is a relevant diversion from the script: the adult plays differently his role than expected by his trainer and this is crucial to interpret and to understand the conversational moves of the child. The adult's interventions influence the child's attitude and understanding of the situation.
Excerpt 3.
Code of videorecording: Clpr3. Pretest 1st grade. Participants: experimenter (Joseph), child (Edy, male; age: 6,11 years old) ((the child has established the equality of the quantities of liquids in glasses A and A'. Then, the adult changes the set of glasses, using A' and B))
Exp.: Your juice will be poured into this new glass ((he pours the juice from glass A' to glass B)) now, what I would like to know is when I drink in my glass and you drink in your glass, do you think that we will have as much or will someone have more juice, will someone have less juice?
Edy: I don't know.
Exp.: You don't know.
Edy: No no.
Exp.: Then I will do what I done in other classes, he was looking at the shape of the glass, the height of the juice, in order to know if there was as much or someone have more juice, someone have less juice.
Edy: Uhm.
Exp.: Maybe we can put the glasses side by side, look at their shapes.
Edy: Well, not the same height.
Exp.: Not the same height.
Edy: No no.
Exp.: Do you think that there will have as much or will someone have more juice, will someone have less juice?
Edy: I don't know.
Exp.: You don't know, you have no idea, maybe you have an idea?
Edy: No.
Exp.: You know that in your glass the juice is higher.
Edy: Ya ya.
((the child has established the equality of quantities of liquid in glasses A and A'. Then, the adult changes the set of glasses, using A and C))
Exp.: I take again another glass, this time I take my juice and I pour it in ((he pours the juice in the glass C)) and now if I drink in this glass and you drink in the other glass, there will have as much or will someone have more juice, will someone have less juice?
Edy: Well, I don't know.
Exp.: You don't know.
Edy: No no.
Exp.: Not a little idea?
Edy: No.
Exp.: If you compare the two glasses side by side.
Edy: Well this one ((glass C)) is more, more round so ((the juice)) then it is, it goes lower down, and then the other one ((glass A)) is smaller on top so it is higher.
Exp.: Ok, as a result of this, it gives you an idea of thequantities of juice, Edy?
Edy: No, euh not especially.
Exp.: Not especially, ok, very well, we have finished, thank you very much.
Excerpt 3 concerns a sequence in which the child answers that he is unable to judge the relative quantities to the experimenter's question about the amount of juice in the glasses (turn 22 «I don't know»). In accordance with the piagetian script, the adult suggests then some alternatives, referring to other (hypothetical) situations in which other children were looking at some particular aspects of the material (turn 25 «the shape of the glass, the height of the juice») to judge the amount, in order to invite the child to make his thought more explicit. However, the intervention of the adult in turn 27 is a diversion from the piagetian script: the adult is explicitly suggesting to the child to compare the two glasses, and especially their shapes. In the eyes of Edy, it is then evident that there is not the same height of juice (turn 28 «well, isn't the same height»), and maybe it is not clear for him why the adult suggested to look at the height of the juice as a possible element to be considered. Within this sequence, the next intervention of the adult in turn 31 (when he is asking again the same question) is an effort to reestablish the main question, but the reaction (turn 32 «I don't know») demonstrates that the child is far from providing the answer expected by the adult. After anoth er attempt of the experimenter (turn 33 «maybe you have an idea»), and the answer of the child (turn 34 «no»), this kind of «escalation» is completed by an inference of the adult: he attributes to the child the recognition of having a higher level of juice (turn 35 «you know that in your glass the juice is higher»), without asking for an explanation, as required by the piagetian script.
In the second part of the excerpt, the same manner of conducting the interview is repeated: when the child in turn 40 states that he doesn't know how to answer the question (even if he is confronted to a different set of glasses), the experimenter tries to propose the comparison of the glasses as a possible solution. Following this suggestion, Edy describes the shapes of the glasses and the level of the juice into each glass (turn 46, the juice «it goes lower down»). Then, the adult asks if this comparison brings a solution about the quantities of juice: in his diversion from the script, the experimenter is now introducing another possible answer, i.e. that the quantities of juice could be related to the shapes of the glasses. Finally, the child confirms that he cannot answer the question and the experimenter (although in sign of thanks) offers what seems to be a positive feedback to the child (turn 49 «ok, fine»). This, of course, is not prescribed by the script and might confirm the child in his error.
The sequence above shows how the adult's interventions can strongly influence the statements made by the child during the interview and fail to give opportunities to assess the child's individual thought. The adult has repeatedly diverged from the intentions of the piagetian script and consequently induced answers in the child. However, even if trained to interview and to follow the piagetian script in testing the conservation of quantities of liquid, the adult's diversions might be an inevitable condition of the situation: within the frame of the inter action, the adult might be induced to transgress the script also because of the child's reactions. In the following part of the paper we show how the answers of the children can be considered the result of their conversational capacity to adapt themselves to the situation.
A good answer in the eyes of the adult/
Another aspect found in our data concerns the children's capacity to adapt their answers to the adult's questions. In the following excerpt, we observe how the child can propose and develop what she considers a «sufficient» response, in order to produce the «good answer» in the eyes of the adult, not understanding that she is asked to reason aloud. The sequence highlights how some conversational moves of the child are adapted to what she believes are the expectations of the interlocutor.
Excerpt 4.
Code of videorecording's code: Clpr 5. Pretest 1st grade. Participants: experimenter (Mary), child (Daria, female; age: 7,2 year sold) ((the child has established the equality of quantities of liquid in glasses A and A'. Then, the adult changes the set of glasses, using A' and B))
Exp.: I take your juice ((glass A')) and I'm pouring it in this glass. ((glass B)) here we are. and now what do you think, if you drink from your glass and I drink from my glass there will have as much or will someone have more juice, will some one have less juice?
Daria: Someone will have less.
Exp.: Someone will have less, who?
Daria: You.
Exp.: It's me, and how do you know that I have less than you?
Daria: Because it's a bigger glass.
Exp.: So could you have a look, I take your glass, ((glass B)) I pour in that glass, ((glass A')) and now what do you think, you drink in your glass ((glass A')) and I drink in my glass, ((glass A)) there will have as much or will someone have more juice, will someone have less juice?
Daria: I don't know.
Exp.: More or less. what do you think?
Daria: Yeah.
Exp.: We will have as much to drink both of us?
Daria: Yeah, I believe.
Exp.: Ok.
In this excerpt, we consider that the statements are based on the estimated (by the child) sufficient answer expected by the experimenter. In answering to the adult question about the quantities of liquid in the glasses, Daria says that the experimenter has less, because her glass is bigger (turn 26). The adult Mary seems to accept this statement (instead of inviting her to argue), and doesn't provide requests for more specific explanations. She doesn't question the child about the relation between the less quantity of juice and the bigger size of the glass. In this case, the experimenter is deviating from the script and, at the same time, she is probably offering a positive feedback to the child.
In the second part of the excerpt, the adult pours back B in A' and asks the child to compare the quantities in A and A' (turn 27). When Daria says that she does not know if there is the same amount in these two similar glasses, the experimenter asks a very general question (turn 29 «more or less. what do you think»), without soliciting the child with more specific questions. As a consequence, the child limits herself by saying «yeah» (turn 30), and «yeah, I think» (turn 32), but without giving any extra explanations. It seems that, within this interaction, the adult is not able to manage the situation, and the child limits her statements on the estimated sufficient answer requested by the test. This «acknowledgement» of a minimal answer is quite difficult to interpret: probably, the experimenter under stands that the quantities of liquid were not equal, because the child was declaring the inequality before, and this influences the conversation and the adult's capacity to properly manage the interview. The adult seems not to have understood the piagetian goal of eliciting reasoning from the child and keeps asking for answers.
Discussion and concluding perspectives.
Our main idea has been to look at the reasoning of the children not only as a sign of concrete operations to back up conservation judgements, but also as the fruit of a coordination with the adult.
The excerpts presented are just three examples of how adults and children construct turn by turn their interventions and how Piaget's intentions can be misunderstood. We have found that sometimes participants follow some implicit and/or explicit suggestions of the partner, and implicit assumptions; sometimes they try to understand which kind of object of discourse is at stake; and sometimes they produce just the estimated sufficient conversational move, in order to provide the right answer in the eyes of the partner. We have observed diversions from the script, adult's inferences and attributions to the child, and the participants' tendency to repeat an interactive pattern (or repeat an answer that he or she thought successful). It is useful to pay attention to the existing discrepancies between the intentions of the script and what really happens. Piaget suggests to confront children with different points of view, and not only to simply ask them to answer questions, in order to have access to their modes of reasoning. The data we have analyzed confirm that the conversation has been «squeezed» into a matter of answers and not a shared reasoning or an argumentation.
In the line of revisiting the test of conservation of quantities of liquid, we think that a more specific attention is crucial to understand the specific forms taken by the conversations if we want to access argumentation. In this sense, further specific analytical efforts will be useful, in order to consider argumentation not just as a symptom of the logical structure of the child's thought, but also as a product of the interactants' conversations. Referring to previous studies on the relevance of the context within the adult-child interactions, we consider very crucial to turn back to the role of the adult: we think that some evidence coming from our study might be considered in order to pay more attention to the delicate and difficult role of the experimenter. In order to open a space of investigation in which the adult can be considered not just as a tester, but as a player during the conversation, we have to look at both interlocutors as responsible for the outcome of the interaction.
In the long term, we consider that multiple extensions of this study can provide a better understanding of how to design settings of interaction between adults and children in order to really have the opportunity to improve argumentation skills and to analyze the argumentation processes.
References
argumentation piagetian test
1. Baucal A., Stepanovic I. Conservation or conversation: a test of the repeated question hypothesis // Psihologija, 39. 2006.
2. Donaldson M. Children's minds. New York, 1978.
3. Duranti A., Goodwin C. (еds.) Rethinking context. Language as an interactive phenomenon. Cambridge: Cam bridge University Press, 1992.
4. Eemeren van F.H., Grootendorst R.A Systematic Theory of Argumentation: the Pragma Dialectical Account. Cam bridge, 2004.
5. Erduran S., Osborne J.F., Simon S. Enhancing the quality of argument in school science // Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41 (10), 2004.
6. Golder C., Schneuwly B. (Eds.) Argumentation in texts: developmental studies in production and comprehension // Argumentation, 10(2), 1996.
7. Greco Morasso S. Argumentative and other communicative strategies of the mediation practice. Unpublioshed Ph.D. thesis. University of Lugano. 2008.
8. Grossen M. La construction sociale de l'intersubjectivite entre adulte et enfant en situation de test. Cousset: Del Val, 1998.
9. Johnson D.W., Johnson R.T. Collaborative Learning and Argumentation. London: Cassel Education, 1994.
10. Krstic K., Baucal A. Symmetrical social relation as a factor in conservation tasks // Psihologija, 36 (4), 2003.
11. Kuhn D. The skills of argument. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1991.
12. Light P. Context, conservation and conversation // M. Richard, P. Light (еds.). Children of social worlds. Development in a social context Cambridge: Polity Press, 1986.
13. Light P., Gorsuch C., Newmann J. Why do you ask? Context and communication in the conservation task // European Journal of Psychology of Education, 2, 1987.
14. Light P., PerretClermont A.N. Social context effects in learning and testing. In A. Gellatly, D. Rogers, J.A. Sloboda (Eds.), Cognition and Social Worlds. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 1989.
15. MarroClement P., Trognon A., PerretClermont A.N. Processus interlocutoires dans une tache de conservation des liq uides // M. Gilly, J.P. Roux, A. Trognon (еds.)., Apprendre dans l'interaction. Nancy: Presses Universitaires de Nancy, 1999.
16. Muller Mirza N., Baucal A., PerretClermont A.N., Marro P. Nice designed experiment goes to the local commu nity // Cahiers de Psychologie, 38, 2003.
17. Muller Mirza N., PerretClermont A.N. (еds.). Argumentation and education. Theoretical Foundations and Practices. New York: Springer, 2009.
18. Nicolet M. Dynamiques relationnelles et processus cognitifs: etude du marquage social chez des enfants de 5 a 9 ans.` Lausanne: Delachaux et Niestle, 1995.
19. Perret-Clermont A.N. Comments on Rigotti and Rocci // Studies in Communication Sciences, 6 (2), 2006.
20. Piaget J. Le jugement et le raisonnement chez l'enfant. Neuchatel: Delachaux et Niestle, 1924.
21. Piaget J. La representation du monde chez l'enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, 1926.
22. Piaget J., Inhelder B. La psychologie de l'enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, 1966.
23. Piaget J., Szeminska A. La genese du nombre chez l'en fant. Neuchatel: Delachaux et Niestle, 1941.
24. Pontecorvo C. Discussing for reasoning: the role of argu ment in knowledge construction // E. De Corte, H. Lodewijks, C. Parmentier, P. Span (еds.). Learning and instruction: A pub lication of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction. Vol. I. Oxford: EARLI, 1987.
25. Pontecorvo C. Thinking with others: the social dimen sion of learning in families and schools // A.N. Perret Clermont, C. Pontecorvo, L. Resnick, T. Zittoun, B. Burge (еds.). Joining Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2004.
26. Pontecorvo C., Arcidiacono F. Famiglie all'italiana: par lare a tavola. Milan: Cortina, 2007.
27. Psaltis C., Duveen G. Social relations and cognitive devel opment: the influence of conversation types and representations of gender // European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 2006.
28. Rigotti E. Relevance of contextbound loci to topical poten tial in the argumentation stage // Argumentation, 20 (4), 2006.
29. Rigotti E., Greco Morasso S. Argumentation as an object of interest and as a social and cultural resource // N. Muller Mirza, A.N. PerretClermont (еds.). Argumentation and edu cation. Theoretical Foundations and Practices. New York: Springer, 2009.
30. Rigotti E., Rocci A. Towards a definition of communication context. Foundations of an interdisciplinary approach to commu nication // Studies in Communication Sciences, 6 (2), 2006.
31. Rommetveit R. On the architecture of intersubjectivity // L.H. Strickland, K. J. Gergen, F.J. Aboud (Eds.), Social Psychology in Transition. New York: Plenum Press, 1976.
32. Rose S.A., Blank M. The potency of context in chil dren's cognition: an illustration through conversation // Child Development, 45, 1974.
Размещено на Allbest.ru
...Подобные документы
Studies by Fischer and his colleagues and Dawson (2006) have investigated development in a wide range of domains, including understanding of social interaction concepts such as "nice" and "mean", skills in mathematics, and understanding "leadership".
реферат [20,2 K], добавлен 22.12.2009The study of harm to children from watching American cartoons. Problem of imitating negative or mindless characters from cartoons. Leading role of American cartoon industry in the animation history. First steps in the progress of a child’s development.
эссе [16,3 K], добавлен 11.04.2013This article suggests specific ways in which college teachers can foster relationships with students that promote motivation and satisfaction. Fostering personal relationships with students. Motivating students to work. Handling interpersonal issues.
статья [18,6 K], добавлен 10.05.2014What is conflict. As there is a conflict. Main components of the conflict. The conflict is a dispute over what. How to resolve the conflict. Negotiations search consent of a compromise. Subject of the dispute. The decision brought. Suppressed discontent.
презентация [50,7 K], добавлен 21.03.2014Unhealthy food, lack of sleep, passive lifestyle, related works. Survey, Passive Lifestyle, Lack Of Sleep, Nutrition. How often pupils have negative feelings. Teachers' complaints. Can we do to reduce these negative displays of pupil’s behavior.
курсовая работа [25,5 K], добавлен 18.05.2015Definition of Leadership. Trait theory. How this theory works. Origin and Analysis and basics Pre-conditions for effective use of Trait theory. Inborn leadership characteristics. Process of impact and interaction among the leader and his followers.
реферат [436,9 K], добавлен 24.09.2014Social interaction and social relation are identified as different concepts. There are three components so that social interaction is realized. Levels of social interactions. Theories of social interaction. There are three levels of social interactions.
реферат [16,8 K], добавлен 18.01.2009Definition the certain latent high temperature of evaporation of the liquid using capital equipment and calculations. The accepted value of the latent high temperature of evaporation. The uncertainty for the specific latent heat of vaporization.
лабораторная работа [247,8 K], добавлен 29.12.2010The geographic characteristics of the Novaya Zemlya archipelago in the Arctic Ocean. The history of the test site at Novaya Zemlya. Learning the facts about the nuclear test site. Description of the scope and consequences of the explosion of King-bomb.
презентация [1,2 M], добавлен 18.10.2015The subjective aspects of social life. Social process – those activities, actions, operations that involve the interaction between people. Societal interaction – indirect interaction bearing on the level of community and society. Modern conflict theory.
реферат [18,5 K], добавлен 18.01.2009Cash flow test and balance sheet test. The reasons of not having a single test. The definition and treatment of the debts and liabilities under the both tests. Some differences between Maltese law and UK law in this question.
реферат [10,7 K], добавлен 11.09.2006Development of the calculation procedures. Initial values of liquid density and dynamic viscosity of crude oil-gas mixes components at 200C and 0.1 MPa. Chart of experimental conditions and properties of crude oil saturated with natural gas samples.
статья [78,1 K], добавлен 21.03.2012Аналіз програмного забезпечення для проведення тестування в комп’ютерному класі. УТК (Універсальний тестовий комплекс). Асистент 2. OPEN TEST. Порівняння програм для тестування. Організація інтерактивного тестування за допомогою програми OPEN TEST.
реферат [30,3 K], добавлен 19.09.2008Test of strength and endurance. The Kazakh national game "Kokpar". Test of strength, agility, accuracy, ability to stay in the saddle. Audaryspak-known form of the Kazakh people's struggle - the forms of struggle riders on horseback. Players in pairs.
презентация [842,1 K], добавлен 11.03.2014Conversation for its own sake. Сonversation for some other purpose. To be successful one has to be a good conversationalist. And the art of conversation is a skill and can be acquired as many other work skills as typing.
топик [4,6 K], добавлен 22.08.2006Principles of learning and language learning. Components of communicative competence. Differences between children and adults in language learning. The Direct Method as an important method of teaching speaking. Giving motivation to learn a language.
курсовая работа [66,2 K], добавлен 22.12.2011Investigation of the process of translation and its approaches. Lexical Transformations, the causes and characteristics of transformation; semantic changes. The use of generic terms in the English language for description specific objects or actions.
курсовая работа [38,0 K], добавлен 12.06.2015
краткое изложение [4,7 K], добавлен 00.00.0000The properties of the proton clusters in inelastic interactions SS. Relativistic nuclear interaction. Studying the properties of baryon clusters in a wide range of energies. Seeing the high kinetic energy of the protons in the rest of the cluster.
курсовая работа [108,6 K], добавлен 22.06.2015Аналіз розвитку ринку відеореклами в Україні, специфіка музичних інструментів і професійного звукорежисерського устаткування. Технологія відеомонтажу і роботи зі звуком у процесі створення рекламного ролику. Використання комплексу Avid Liquid 7 Pro.
дипломная работа [3,0 M], добавлен 03.01.2011