Impossible Phenomena as Mediators in Cognitive Functioning and Education
Role of impossible phenomena (entities and events) as psychological tools for enhancing cognitive functioning. Improvement in the abilities after watching a film with impossible phenomena than after watching a film with contrasting possible phenomena.
Рубрика | Психология |
Вид | статья |
Язык | английский |
Дата добавления | 06.04.2019 |
Размер файла | 2,6 M |
Отправить свою хорошую работу в базу знаний просто. Используйте форму, расположенную ниже
Студенты, аспиранты, молодые ученые, использующие базу знаний в своей учебе и работе, будут вам очень благодарны.
Размещено на http://www.allbest.ru/
Impossible Phenomena as Mediators in Cognitive Functioning and Education
Субботский Евгений Васильевич
доктор психологических наук
Аннотация
Предметом обсуждения является использование образов невозможных феноменов (объектов и событий) как средств для улучшения когнитивного функционирования и обучения. В двух исследованиях участникам (детям школьного возраста) показывали фильм, содержащий возможные или невозможные (магические) объекты и события, а затем тестировали на креативное мышление и на способность отличать фантастические образы от реалистических. В третьем исследовании участников (подростков и взрослых) тестировали на способность запоминать коммерческие бренды вставленные в контекст рекламных роликов, содержащих невозможные феномены или равно интересные, но возможные феномены. Во всех трех исследованиях участники продемонстрировали более высокий уровень когнитивного функционирования (мышления, восприятия и памяти) после просмотра фильмов с участием невозможных феноменов чем после просмотра фильмов с участием равно интересных и увлекательных но возможных феноменов. Обсуждены перспективы использования образов невозможных феноменов для оптимизации обучения в школе. Методом исследования была оценка воздействия фильмов с участием возможных либо невозможных феноменов на когнитивное функционирование: креативное мышление, восприятие и память. Новизна: (1) Впервые образы невозможных (магических) феноменов рассматриваются как способ воздействия на когнитивные функции (мышление, восприятие и память) с целью оптимизации когнитивного функционирования; (2) Впервые рассмотрена возможность создания альтернативных учебников по различным предметам, в которых законы природы не соблюдались бы, а нарушались, и оценена роль невозможных феноменов для повышения интереса учеников к обучению научным предметам. Выводы: (1) Демонстрация невозможных феноменов в контексте фильма повышает способности детей к креативному мышлению и к умению различать фантастические образы от реалистических; (2) Включение рекламных продуктов в контекст невозможных феноменов повышает запоминаемость этих продуктов; (3) Образы невозможных феноменов могут быть эффективно использованы для повышения интереса к усвоению знаний в школе.
Ключевые слова: когнитивное функционирование, память, восприятие, креативное мышление, магические эффекты, невозможные феномены, обучение, альтернативные учебники, невозможные объекты, реклама
psychological cognitive ability impossible
Abstract
The manuscript assesses the role of impossible phenomena (entities and events) as psychological tools for enhancing cognitive functioning. In three studies, participants were exposed to films containing either impossible or contrasting possible phenomena, and then tested on creativity, the ability to distinguish fantasy from reality, and the ability to memorize commercial brands placed within films. In all of the studies, participants (British 4-, 6- and 8-year-old children, adolescents and adults) showed a significantly better improvement in the aforementioned abilities after watching the film with impossible phenomena than after watching the film with contrasting possible phenomena. For the first time the images of impossible (fantasy) phenomena is being viewed as the means to affect cognitive abilities (thoughts, perception, and memory) for the purpose of optimizing cognitive functionality. The results are discussed in terms of possible applications of impossible phenomena in a classroom.
Keywords: cognitive functioning, memory, perception, creative thinking, magical effects, impossible phenomena, training, alternative textbooks, impossible objects, advertising
There are images, such as the “impossible triangle” by an English mathematician Roger Penrose or the “impossible landscapes” crafted by a Dutch graphic artist Maurits Escher, which violate known laws of geometry and gravity yet look amazingly real. But why do scientists and artists create the impossible images in the first place? Are these images a mere curiosity, or might there be more to them? In this paper I am going to argue that the impossible images can be used as tools, or mediators for teaching and communication.
Mediation is one of key features of human development. According to Vygotsky, a person is connected to the world in two parallel ways: directly and indirectly, via mediators -- objects that are used to amplify the connection between the person and the world [1] These special objects, or mediators, perform multiple functions, such as upgrading innate cognitive abilities to the higher social forms, providing the mind with a symbolic “operational field”, and others [2][3].The function that is of special interest for the present analysis is the ability of mediators to enhance, or amplify, human actions. Building a ship with bare hands (an unmediated action) is more difficult and less efficient than building a ship with the assistance of tools (a mediated action), and communication by gaze referencing (an unmediated action) is less precise and powerful than communication by language (a mediated action). Thus, speaking of the development of attention in the child, Vygotsky writes “With the help of the indicatory function of words, ...he (the child - ES) begins to take control over his attention…distinguishing more and more new patterns from the background … and thus infinitely widening the ability to guide his attention” [4].
Mediators, (sometimes called “psychological tools”), include a wide variety of objects, ranging from ordinary tools (such as a hammer) to semiotic cultural artifacts (signs, symbols, texts, formulae, and language) [5][6][7][8]. Action amplification through mediation is not an exclusively human ability. Apes can use tools similar to those used by humans to amplify their actions: stones for cracking nuts, rods for fishing aunts, sounds and abstract images to enhance and direct their communications [9]. The principle of action amplification through mediation is so common that it almost became a triviality. Thus, efficacy of teaching can be assessed by efficacy of tools involved to amplify the teaching process, such as handbooks, teaching methods and teacher-student communication styles, and a position of a certain species of animals on the ladder of phylogenetic development can be defined by the animals' ability to use tools.
There is, however, a kind of mediators less ordinary, which is unique to humans. This kind of mediators is images of objects and events, which are impossible. In this paper, by the impossible phenomena (“the IP”) I will refer to images of entities and events that violate known laws of science. The Penrose triangle, animals that can speak human languages, a wizard creating a castle by a magic spell are examples of the IP. The specific feature of the IP is that such entities exist only in one's imagination (thinking, dreaming) and do not have corresponding referents in the real world. Along with the IP, in this paper the concept of contrasting possible phenomena (“the P”) will be employed. By the P I will refer to images of entities and events, which are the exact opposite to the IP. By definition, the P can exist both in imagination and in reality. For instance, for the IP such as “an apple flying upwards by itself” the P is “an apple falling down on earth”, for the IP such as a “half man-half horse” the P are “a man” and “a horse”.
The main question I am going to discuss in this paper can be put as follows: Are the IP a more efficient tool of amplifying cognition and action than are the P? Fiction literature and cinema are by far the most popular mediators for influencing human behavior. Recent experiments in neuroscience indicated that reading to participants clips from “Harry Potter” that included the IP activated areas in the participants' brain different from those activated by reading clips that included the P [10]. It is therefore a relevant question to ask, which of the two kinds of mediators - the P or the IP - if framed within a film or a TV program, would affect children's subsequent behavior to a greater extent. For example, is it more efficient to teach children the law of gravity by demonstrating them a movie in which a horse is falling down from a cliff (the P), or a movie in which a winged horse is flying up in the air (the IP)?
Regarding the aforementioned example, two alternative hypotheses can be put forward. Hypothesis 1 suggests that the P are better amplifiers of actions than the IP, because the P are embedded in children's everyday experience and the IP are not (the P/IP hypothesis). For instance, in the everyday life children often see animals falling down from heights, whereas large animals flying up in the air (i.e., dragons) are a rare phenomenon that can only be seen in movies or dreams. Alternatively, Hypothesis 2 contends that the amplifying power of the IP should be greater than that of the P (the IP/P hypothesis), due to the “attention grabbing” nature of the IP [11]. Indeed, when a person is observing the P (i.e., a common horse), all that this image activates is the person's memories about the horse. In contrast, when a person is observing the IP (i.e., a winged horse), this image activates the person's memories about the winged horse. In addition, the person is aware that such an entity is impossible. This awareness, by association, activates the person's memories about the P (i.e., the images of a common horse and a bird) (see Fig. 1). Because of this, when the IP is used as a mediator, its action amplifying power is
Fig. 1 Imagining possible versus impossible entities added to by the action amplifying power of the P (see Fig. 2).
Fig. 2. Action amplifying capacity of possible (P) versus impossible (IP) entities.
In this paper, three studies are reviewed, which compared the amplifying effect of the IP and the P on various types of cognitive functioning: creative thinking, perception and memory.
Comparing the amplifying effects of possible and impossible phenomena on cognitive functioning
The first study compared the efficacy of the IP and the P as facilitators of children's creative thinking [12]. Creativity is typically defined as the ability to generate “novel behavior that meets a standard of quality and/or utility” [13, p. 308]. On a certain kind of tasks creativity takes shape of divergent thinking - the ability to solve problems that have not one correct solution but a variety of alternative solutions. On these divergent thinking tasks , thinking about the P provides multiple realistic solutions (i.e., one can move from one place to another by using a car, a train, or a plain), and thinking about the IP provides multiple unrealistic (impossible) solutions (i.e., one can move from one place to another by using a broomstick, a magic carpet, or on a dragon). The common feature (divergent thinking) makes it possible to expect that engaging children in one of these activities--thinking about the IP--might enhance the other activity--creativity of realistic thinking, through prompting and (or) association.
Showing children a movie was chosen as a method of engaging the children in thinking about the P or the IP because the developmental research had shown that exposure to cinema and TV affects children's subsequent behavior [14][15]. Cosequently, the study's aim was to find out which of the two movie clips - the one that contains the P (the P film) or the one that contains the IP (the IP film) - would make observers to subsequently think more creatively in solving divergent thinking tasks.
Accordingly, in Experiment 1 of this study British children aged 4 and 6 years were divided into experimental and control groups. Children of the experimental groups were shown the IP film, and children of the control groups were shown the P film. Both films included clips taken from the original movie “Harry Potter and the Philosopher's Stone”. The IP film contained impossible phenomena such as people riding broomsticks, animals turning into humans, and people becoming invisible. The P film included clips from the same movie, with the same characters doing similar actions (communicating, moving around, and doing things), however, no impossible phenomena happened. Independent experts rated the movies for such features, as impossibility of phenomena, pace, emotional attractiveness, and visual and sound effects. On all of these features, except the impossibility of phenomena, the films were rated as approximately identical.
Creativity was assessed on Torrance's [16] “Creativity in Action and Movement” test, and on the modified version of Karmiloff-Smith's [17] “Drawing the Impossible Entities” test. Both inventories contained divergent thinking tasks. On some subtests of these inventories children were tested before exposure to the films, and on the remaining subtests they were tested after exposure to the films. The results indicated that after, but not before exposure to the films, children in the experimental groups scored significantly higher than controls on the majority of TCAM subtests (see Fig. 3). The same tendency was observed in the drawing test.
Fig. 3. Mean summary scores on TCAM (fluency, originality and imagination) as a function of the type of Film (with impossible versus possible effects), and age (4 and 6 years) in Experiment 1.
The aim of Experiment 2 of this study was to replicate the data of Experiment 1, with different groups of children aged 6 and 8 years, coming from a different county of England, and with a different experimenter. Another aim was to find out whether watching the IP film, along with facilitating children's creativity, would also increase the children's belief in that the IP are in fact real. The procedure of this experiment was the same as in Experiment 1, except that, after exposure to the films, the children were also given a questionnaire, which tested their belief in reality of the IP [18]. As in Experiment 1, in Experiment 2, after but not before watching the films, in the experimental groups children showed significantly greater creativity scores than in the control groups (Fig. 4).
Fig. 4. Mean summary scores on TCAM (fluency, originality and imagination) as a function of the type of Film (with impossible versus possible effects), and age (6 and 8 years) in Experiment 2.
Interestingly, there were no significant differences found between experimental and control groups on children's belief in reality of impossible phenomena. Altogether, both experiments supported the IP/P hypothesis, according to which exposing children to the IP film facilitated their creativity to a significantly stronger extent than exposing them to the P film. Simultaneously, it was shown that exposing children to the IP does not increase their belief in that the IP are real.
In another study, the effect of watching the IP or the P films on children's ability to discriminate fantastic from realistic visual displays was investigated [19]. The ability to distinguish fantasy from reality appears early in child development [20][21]. This ability mediates the effect of mass media on children's subsequent behavior. For example, children who believed a violent film clip was a documentary later reacted more aggressively compared to children who believed the film was fantasy [22][23]. Аs in the previous study, in this study the aim was to find out which kind of film - the IP or the P - would facilitate children's ability to discriminate fantasy from reality to a greater extent. It was hypothesized that exposing children to the IP would have a greater effect, due to the contrasting nature of human thinking. Indeed, seeing a picture of an impossible entity (i.e., a flying elephant) would make children imagine contrasting possible entities (i.e., a common elephant and a bird), and thus reflect upon the difference between impossible (fantastic) and possible (realistic) images, whereas seeing a picture of a possible entity (i.e., a common elephant) would not.
Participants were 6- and 9- year old children from a primary school in England. The same films were employed as in the previous study. Children were randomly divided into experimental and control groups. In the experimental groups children were shown the IP, and in the control groups children were shown the P. Before and after exposure to the films, the children were offered a computer interactive test on their ability to distinguish between possible and impossible visual images. The images were photos of the great paintings or their fragments. In a test trial, the child was presented a display, which had a target picture on top and two test pictures at the bottom. The pictures where showing fantastic or realistic images. The child was instructed to pick one of the pictures at the bottom that matched the target picture on top in regard to whether it was showing a possible or an impossible entity or event. There were 42 test trials altogether.
The results indicated that after, but not before exposure to the films, the children who watched the IP obtained significantly higher mean scores on correct identifications than children who watched the P (Fig.5). This result supported the IP/P hypothesis: children's exposure to impossible phenomena
Fig. 5. Mean number of correct identifications (out of 42) as a function of Group (experimental vs control) and Time of testing (before vs, after exposure to the films) enhanced the children's ability to discriminate fantastic pictures from realistic ones to a significantly greater extent than exposure to equally interesting and colorful but realistic phenomena.
Finally, an attempt was made to investigate whether the IP/P effect is also true for memory [24]. For that purpose, commercial TV advertisements were used. The aim of the study was to examine whether framing advertised products in the context of the IP could facilitate the viewers' memory for these products to the same, smaller or greater extent than framing the products in the context of the P. Indeed, memory experts have long advocated that bizarre imagery facilitates learning through reduced interference (“bizarreness effect”) [25][26]. The IP are implausible and can therefore be described as bizarre. Studies have shown that placing commercial products within films can elicit successful recognition and recall of the advertised products through cues and association [27][28]. It was assumed in this study that marketing companies frame their advertised products in the context of the IP because they intuitively believe that the use of impossible phenomena makes viewers remember these products better.
To investigate whether this belief is true or false, British adolescents and adults were shown the P and the IP films, which contained television advertisements. The participants were asked to recall and recognize the films' advertised products. The IP included advertising brands such as Mini Cooper, Levi, Paco Rabanne and Pepsi Maxx framed in the context of impossible phenomena (i.e., talking animals or inanimate objects coming to life), and the P showed clips with similar brands framed in the context of equally interesting and exiting but possible phenomena. The films were matched according to other dimensions, such as pace, action and emotional content. After watching the films, participants completed a general recall test and a recognition test. The recognition test included 18 brands, 9 of which had been shown in the film, and the other 9 were used as distractors.
The results showed that there was no difference on general recall between the IP and the P films; however, on the recognition test significant differences were obtained. On the immediate recognition, adolescents recognized significantly larger number of brands from the IP than from the P, but there was no such difference in adults (Fig.6). However, on delayed recognition two weeks later, the reverse results were obtained: adults, but not adolescents, showed the IP/P effect (Fig. 7).
Fig. 6. Mean number of correctly recognised brands on immediate testing, as a function of Film (with impossible vs possible effects) and Age (adolescents vs adults)
Fig. 7. Mean number of correctly recognised brands on delayed testing, as a function of Film (with impossible vs possible effects) and Age (adolescents vs adults)
Overall, the experiment's results have shown that brands' inclusion into the context of the IP does indeed provide a better memory on these brands compared with brands included into the context of the P. However, this improvement occurs not in participants' explicit memory tested via general recall, but in implicit memory tested via recognition of brands.
Discussion
It is natural for humans to think in comparisons. Whenever we think of something (i.e., about nice weather), we inevitably also think of something, which makes a contrast to it (i.e., bad weather). In most cases we are not aware of the fact that we think through contrasts, but we do.
Teachers sometimes take advantage of this feature of human thinking. For example, “compare and contrast” is a popular strategy used in a classroom in order to make students to comprehensively ponder and analyse a topic. Psychologists compared various forms of effective instruction at a classroom; they discovered that comparative analysis is superior to other forms of analyses and has the greatest effect on students' achievements [29][30]. In children's literature, books that use contrast for education are in the hundreds. In Raymond Briggs' “Fungus the Bogeyman” [31], which is part of Year 2 curriculum in British education, the characters Bogeymen have a strange anatomy, sleep in dirty beds, keep their cloths and shoes in cold water and do all things unlike humans. In Roald Dahl's [32] “The Twits”, the characters are people, not fungus, but they too do things in the most odd way. Using the IP can be viewed as a case of teaching through contrasts, with the aim of helping children to reflect upon and analyse known laws of physics and other sciences.
Even 3-year-olds show some realization that fantastic objects are less real than perceived or imagined physical objects [20][33]. Five-year-olds older children and are as good as adults are at distinguishing between fantastic and real objects [34]. When children of this age watch cartoons or TV advertisements, they become sensitive to violation of physical laws. Thus, the author observed a 7 year old boy who, when seeing a commercial in which washing machines are flying, commented “Washing machines can not fly”, and even the adult's subsequent remark that the washing machines actually have small propellers attached to them could not reassure the child.
The aforementioned example highlights one of the psychological mechanisms that constitute the IPs amplifying power - the awareness through contrast. Three studies reviewed in this paper confirmed the hypothesis, according to which watching the IP enhances cognitive functioning to a significantly greater extent than watching the P. The IP/P effect worked in regard to various cognitive functions - creative thinking, perception and memory. The psychological mechanism behind this effect is hidden in the “attention grabbing” nature of the IP. Using this mechanism provides educationalists with an exciting opportunity to discuss scientific problems with children in a manner, which is free from didactic pressure and boredom of a traditional teaching style. Ultimately, this line of research can lead towards constructing special “alternative handbooks” on geometry, physics, biology, psychology and other subjects, in which events do not conform to the laws of nature, but violate these laws. This kind of handbooks would not replace the traditional ones, rather it would make a useful supplement to them, helping students better reflect upon the laws of science, understand and memorise these laws. As a matter of fact, this kind of “handbooks” already exists, albeit not in a printed form.
In some books, movies and computer games known properties of solid objects, light, gravity, animal and human psychology and biology are suspended. Lewis Carroll's “Alice's Adventures in Wonderland” in a classic example, and J. K. Rowling's “Harry Potter is a more recent one. Thus, in the Harry Potter series, when Harry and his companions go through the solid wall at the railway station, to get into the magical side of the world, questions can be asked: Why other people in the station can't do the same? If the wall can let people like Harry and their carts through, why can't it let the light through at the same time? When, in another episode, Harry puts on a mantle that makes him invisible, other people can't see him, but can he see other people? If he can, then the bottoms of his eyes should reflect light and be visible to others. If he can't, he is blind and somebody could stumble upon him. When students of the Hogwarts School are riding the broomsticks, do they have weight or do they become weightless? If they have weight, how can they keep balance on a wooden stick without support when turning at sharp angles at a high speed? And if they are weightless, why do they sometimes fall from the broomsticks? When a python in the zoo communicates with Harry, does he understand what Harry says, or is he reading Harry's mind directly? This kind of discussions can enrich and complement the direct instruction on the laws of science and help children better understand and remember these laws.
Библиография
1. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambrdige: Harvard University Press.
2. Kozulin, A. (2001). ?Psychological Tools?: ?A Sociocultural Approach to ???????????????????????????????????????????? Education?. Harvard, Massachusettes: Harvard University Press.
3. Wertsch, J. (1991). ?Voices of the Mind?: ?Sociocultural Approach to Mediated Action?.????????????????????????????????????????????????Cambridge, Massachusettes: Harvard University Press.
4. Vygotsky, L. S. (1984). Tool and sign in child development. In Vygotsky, L.S., “Collected works”, V.6, Moscow: Pedagogica.
5. Bodrova, E. & Leong, D. (1996). ?Tools of the mind?: ?the Vygotskian ??????????????????????????????????????approach to early ????????????childhood education?. New York: Merill
6. Cole, M. (1996). Cultural Psychology: A once and future discipline. Boston, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
7. Engestrom, Y. (2007). Enriching the Theory of Expansive Learning: Lessons From Journeys Toward Coconfiguration. Mind, Culture and Activity, 14, 23-39.
8. Frawley, W. (1997). ?Vygotsky and cognitive science?: ?language and the unification of ???????????????the???????????????????????????? social and computational mind?. Harward, Massachusettes: Harvard University Press.
9. Rumbaugh, D.M., Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S., & Sevcik, R.A. (1994). Behavioral roots of language: A comparative perspective of chimpanzee, child, and culture. In R.W.Wrangham, W.C.McGrew, F.B.M. de Waal, & P.R. Heltne (Eds.), Chimpanzee cultures. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
10. Hsu, C.T, Jacobs, A.M., Altmann, U., & Conrad, M. (2015). The magical activation of left amygdala when reading Harry Potter: an fMRI study on how descriptions of supra-natural events entertain and enchant. PloS ONE, 1.
11. Boyer, P. & Walker, S.(2000). Intuitive ontology and cultural input in the acquisition of religious concepts. In Rosengren, K. S., Johnson, C. N., & Harris, P. L. (Eds), Imagining the impossible. Magical, scientific and religious thinking in children (pp. 130-156). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
12. Subbotsky, E., Hysted, C., & Jones, N. (2010). Watching films with magical content facilitates creativity in children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 111, 261-277.
13. Eisenberger, R., Haskins, F., & Gambleton, P. (1999). Promised reward and creativity: Effects of prior experience. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 308-325.
14. Huston, A. C., & Wright, J. C. (1997) Mass media and children's development. In W.Damon (Gen. Ed.), I. E.Sigel, & K. A. Reninger (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol.4. Child psychology in practice. New York: Wiley, 999-1058.
15. Singer, D., & Singer, J. (Eds.) (2001) Handbook of children and the media. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
16. Torrance, E.P. (1981). Thinking creatively in action and movement. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.
17. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1989). Constrainst on representational change: Evidence from children's drawings. Cognition, 34, 57-83.
18. Bolton, D., Dearsley, P., Madronal-Luque, R., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). Magical thinking in childhood and adolescence: Development and relation to obsessive compulsion. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 479-494.
19. Subbotsky, E., & Slater, E. (2011). Children's discrimination of fantastic vs realistic visual displays after watching a film with magical content. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 112, 603-609.
20. Harris, P. L., Brown, E., Marriott, C., Whittall, S. and Harmer, S. (1991). Monsters, ghosts andwitches: Testing the limits of the fantasy-reality distinction in young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 105-23.
21. Woolley, J.D. (1997). Thinking about fantasy: Are children fundamentally different thinkers and believers from adults? Child Development, 68, 991-1011.
22. Atkin, C. (1983) Effects of realistic TV violence vs. fictional violence on aggression. Journalism Quarterly, 60, 615-621.
23. Comstock, G., & Scharrer, E. (2006). Media and pop culture. In W.Damon & R.M.Lerner (Senes Eds.) and K.A.Reninger & I.Sigel (Vol.Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol.4, 6th ed.), New York: Wiley, pp. 817-863.
24. Subbotsky, E., & Matthews, J. (2011). Magical thinking and memory: Distinctiveness effect in for TV advertisements with magical content. Psychological Reports, 109, 1-11.
25. Wollen, K. A., Weber, A., & Lowry, D. H. (1972). Bizarreness versus interaction of mental images as determinants of learning. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 518-523.
26. Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Lackey, S. (1989). Bizarre imagery, interference, and distinctiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 137-146
27. Babin, L. A., & Garder, S. T. (1996). Viewers' recognition of brands placed within a film. International Journal of Advertising, 15, 140-151.
28. Gupta, P. B., Lord, K. R., 1998. Product placement in movies: The effect of prominence and mode on audience recall. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 20, 47-59.
29. Marzano, R.J. (2007). The art of science and teaching. Alexandria, VA: ASCD Publ.
30. Marzano, R.J., Marzano, J.S, & Pickering, D.J. (2003) Classroom management that works. Research based strategies for every teacher. Alexandria, VA: ASCD Publ.
31. Briggs, R. (2012). Fungus the Bogeyman. Singapore: Penguin Books.
32. Dahl, R. (1980). The Twits. London: Puffin Books.
33. Wellman, H.M., and Estes, D. (1986). Early understanding of mental entities: a reexamination of childhood realism. Child Development, 57, 910-923.
34. Sharon, T., & Wooley, J.D. (2004). Do monsters dream? Young children's understanding of the fantasy/reality distinction. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 293-310.
Размещено на Allbest.ru
...Подобные документы
Influence psychology of cognitive activity and cognitive development on student’s learning abilities during study. Cognitive development theory in psychology. Analysis of Jean Piaget's theory. Her place among the other concept of personal development.
презентация [1,3 M], добавлен 13.04.2016The theme of death in the Gothic novel reality. The Gothic image of the world and its fear of an uncertain and unpredictable universe. The fear as the most eminent theme in Poe’s story "The Tell-Tale Heart". The terrible motives of indistinct phenomena.
лекция [22,4 K], добавлен 01.07.2013Theoretical basis of a role plays as a teaching aid. Historic background of game origin. Psychological value of a role plays. The main function and principles of game organization. Gaming technique. Classification of role plays. Advantages of a game.
курсовая работа [50,7 K], добавлен 26.04.2013The study of harm to children from watching American cartoons. Problem of imitating negative or mindless characters from cartoons. Leading role of American cartoon industry in the animation history. First steps in the progress of a child’s development.
эссе [16,3 K], добавлен 11.04.2013The definition of stress as the body's way of responding to any kind of stimuli. Consideration of positive and negative emotions, which may cause stress. External and internal causes of stress. The role of consciousness in the assessment of events.
презентация [1,1 M], добавлен 22.09.2015The problem of evaluation, self-assessment of personality as a psychological category. Factors of formation evaluation and self-esteem of children of primary school age. An experimental study of characteristics evaluation and self-esteem of junior pupils.
курсовая работа [28,6 K], добавлен 19.05.2011Governmental theory - one of important and perspective directions of modern political ideas. Political sphere from complete. The political phenomena are in structures, prevailing over paradigms in connection with the complex of the public phenomena.
реферат [24,3 K], добавлен 22.11.2010Phonetic coincidence and semantic differences of homonyms. Classification of homonyms. Diachronically approach to homonyms. Synchronically approach in studying homonymy. Comparative typological analysis of linguistic phenomena in English and Russia.
курсовая работа [273,7 K], добавлен 26.04.2012The first "mysterious disappearances" in the Bermuda Triangle. Disappearance about 100 big ships and airplanes the last one hundred years. Other abnormal phenomena of the Bermuda Triangle. The various theories solving secret of the Bermuda Triangle.
презентация [577,8 K], добавлен 23.05.2012The history of the emergence of Hollywood in the central region of Los Angeles, USA. Education on this territory of the first film studios and film industry. "Walk of Fame" and especially its creation. The use of science for the production of films.
презентация [6,5 M], добавлен 18.12.2014It is impossible to discuss a future role of the United States of America in the world without understanding the global processes that have been taken place in the world over the last several years.
сочинение [4,0 K], добавлен 10.03.2006Film "The Humorous Phases of Funny Faces". Creation of characters. Sculptures Base and Woody with the coordinate grid, ready to numbering. Process of animation. Advantages of some programs. Staff from a cartoon film. The imagination and a good software.
презентация [317,6 K], добавлен 07.03.2012А real haunted house is a place that hides many secrets of good and evil, of morality and crimes. Human beings are unable to understand these phenomena because they don't want to accept things that frighten them.
топик [7,9 K], добавлен 09.12.2004The main reasons for and background big disaster, which occurred as a result of the oil spill in the Gulf. Environmental impacts of the spill and its negative impact on the environment. Prevention of these phenomena in the future in the United States.
презентация [440,2 K], добавлен 01.06.2015History and basic steps of creating a film "Help", his theme and content. The reflection in the movie the problems of racial segregation and discrimination based on gender. Characteristics of the main characters and the description of their images.
реферат [16,8 K], добавлен 19.06.2013Essence of humour is and his function. Language and the types, techniques and ways of conveying humourous effect in its various expressions. Phenomena as irony, sarcasm, satire. Difference of irony from other terms, united under the umbrella of humour.
курсовая работа [37,4 K], добавлен 15.12.2011Методика и особенности формирования лексических навыков при изучении иностранного языка. Рекомендации по развитию умений диалогической речи и монологической речи, навыков развития письменной речи с использованием любимых кинофильмов шестиклассников.
конспект урока [181,0 K], добавлен 17.05.2010The differences between the legal norm and the state institutions. The necessity of overcoming of contradictions between the state and the law, analysis of the problems of state-legal phenomena. Protecting the interests and freedoms of social strata.
статья [18,7 K], добавлен 10.02.2015Features of the study and classification of phenomena idiom as a linguistic element. Shape analysis of the value of idioms for both conversational and commercial use. Basic principles of pragmatic aspects of idioms in the field of commercial advertising.
курсовая работа [39,3 K], добавлен 17.04.2011James Cameron is famous canadian film director, screenwriter, producer, and researcher of the underwater world. James Cameron's filmography includes five financial records. Interesting Facts of life and work of film director. History of the personal life.
презентация [3,3 M], добавлен 21.10.2014