Psychological, social and emotional well-being of adults with a history of institutionalization: the pilot study findings
The mental indicators of adults raised in orphanages in comparison with a control group of adults who grew up in biological families. A hypothesis has been put forward about the impact of the current living environment on the diagnosed indicators.
Рубрика | Психология |
Вид | статья |
Язык | английский |
Дата добавления | 17.08.2020 |
Размер файла | 817,2 K |
Отправить свою хорошую работу в базу знаний просто. Используйте форму, расположенную ниже
Студенты, аспиранты, молодые ученые, использующие базу знаний в своей учебе и работе, будут вам очень благодарны.
Размещено на http://www.allbest.ru/
PSYCHOLOGICAL, SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL WELL-BEING OF ADULTS WITH A HISTORY OF INSTITUTIONALIZATION: THE PILOT STUDY FINDINGS
M.A. Chumakovaa, M.A. Zhukovabe, S.A. KORNILOV,
I.V Golovanovab, A.O. Davydovab, T.I. Logvinenkob,
I.V Ovchinnikovab, M.V. Petrovd, A.V. Antonovaa,
O.YU. Naumovaf E.L. Grigorenkobe
a National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russian Federation
b Saint Petersburg State University,Saint Petersburg, Russian Federation
c Institute for Systems Biology, Seattle, USA
d Kashchenko Psychiatric Hospital, Nikolskoye Village, Gatchinsky District, Leningrad Region, Russian Federation
e University of Houston, Houston, USA
f Vavilov Institute of General Genetics, Moscow, Russian Federation
Abstract
The paper presents pilot research data that is part of the research project “The Impact of Early Deprivation on the Bio-Behavioral Indicators of Child Development” from the Government's megagrant programs No. 14.Z50.31.0027. The purpose of the paper is the study of mental, social and emotional well-being indicators of adults raised in orphanages in comparison with a control group of adults who grew up in biological families. The comparison was carried out using scales of the WHO Questionnaire for assessing quality of life and Adult Self-Report (ASR) from The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) for assessing psychological and emotional well-being. Results revealed no significant differences between the groups studied. However, using the classification procedures, we found that an important feature is the living conditions that enable classification of membership in a group of graduates of an orphanage or in a group of adults from biological families: a separate apartment or a “public” space (a communal apartment or a dorm). A hypothesis has been put forward about the impact of the current living environment on the diagnosed indicators of the mental, social and emotional well-being of orphanage graduates.
Keywords: orphans, orphanage, mental, social and emotional well-being.
Резюме
В статье представлены пилотные данные исследования, являющегося частью исследовательского проекта «Влияние ранней депривации на биоповеденческие показатели развития ребенка», осуществляемого в рамках программы мегагрантов Правительства РФ № 14.Z50.31.0027. Цель статьи -- изучение показателей психического, социального и эмоционального благополучия взрослых с опытом институционализации в сравнении с контрольной группой взрослых, выросших в биологических семьях. Сравнение проводилось по шкалам Опросного листа Ахенбаха и краткого опросника ВОЗ для оценки качества жизни. Значимых различий по шкалам опросников между группой выпускников детских домов и контрольной группой обнаружено не было. Однако при использовании процедур классификации было установлено, что важным признаком, позволяющим классифицировать принадлежность к группе выпускников детского дома или к группе взрослых из биологических семей, являются условия проживания: отдельная квартира или «общественное» пространство -- коммунальная квартира или общежитие. Выдвинута гипотеза о влиянии актуальной жизненной среды на диагностируемые показатели психического, социального и эмоционального благополучия выпускников интернатных учреждений.
Ключевые слова: сироты, детский дом, психическое, социальное и эмоциональное благополучие.
Introduction
biological familie orphanages mental indicators
According to survey data collected in the Russian Federation, many children with a history of institutional placement (in orphanages or baby homes) experience problems with social adaptation, due to a lack of social skills and emotion regulation (Bobyleva, 2007). It has been shown that children currently residing in orphanages (Pastukhova, 2011), as well as children who have graduated from and left orphanages (Prisyazhnaya, 2007), experience problems with social adaptation. Children in this group show an increased prevalence of internalizing problems (e.g., aloofness, fear of being alone) and attachment disturbances. Adults with a history of institutionalization often experience problems with employment because of low working motivation; they also demonstrate high rates of substance abuse (Sem'ya, 2007).
The detrimental psychological effects of being raised in institutions without parental care have been vastly discussed and investigated world-wide (e.g., Bos et al., 2011; Erol, Oztop, & Ozcan, 2008; Groze & Ileana, 1996; Merz & McCall, 2010). It has been shown that children and adolescents raised in institutions exhibit marked deficits across a number of developmental domains compared to age- matched children raised in family settings. These include cognitive, affective, and behavioral difficulties and higher rates of such disorders as anxiety, depression, ADHD and conduct disorder (Bos et al., 2011; Erol et al., 2008; Merz & McCall, 2010).
Factors that may influence or contribute to these social maladaptations may include strict routines within the institution, lack of interaction with caregivers, and restricted social activities (Shulga, 2011). These factors contribute to dependent behavior and insecurities about the future, resulting in difficulty in forming and maintaining social connections for adults with a history of institutionalization.
Despite the importance of social adaptation for children and adolescents raised in institutional care, there is currently a dearth of research on the long-lasting residual effects of institutional placement on the life course of orphanage graduates who are actively engaged in social life. This lack of empirical data may be partially explained by the difficulty of organizing such studies: the recruitment of participants is challenging due to the stigma surrounding orphanage graduates; this label holds negative connotations that lead to many concealing their life stories. In addition, information is legally restricted in the Russian Federation concerning adoption and institutional placement.
The long-term impact of institutionalization is a serious concern as people with a history of institutional care (IC) face possible exclusion from social life (Bakhmatova & Chusova, 2013). As a group, adults with a history of institutionalization are characterized by higher, compared to baseline, unemployment rates and criminal records (Sem'ya, 2007). Focus groups have identified the following factors that potential employers reported about adults with an IC history: lack of social skills (passive or aggressive attitudes towards peers; abuse of welfare benefits; avoiding responsibility or shifting responsibility; and substance abuse). In turn, according to surveys conducted with adults with an IC history, these adults perceive employers to have unrealistic expectations, believe they are being treated unequally as a socially disadvantaged group, and report that they are negatively labeled as poor or thieves or delinquents (Ibid.). Thus, employers avoid employing individuals who have an IC history, and people with an IC history are not looking for full-time jobs; this makes social adaptation for adults with an IC challenging (Bakhmatova & Chusova, 2013).
To address this problem a number of policies have been introduced in Russia aimed at increasing the social adaptation of people with an IC history (Sem'ya, 2007). Yet, these policies are not evidence-based, as this group of adults is poorly represented in the research literature (Shulga, 2011). The lack of empirical data on adults with an IC history is evident not only in Russia, but also elsewhere in the world. Institutional care is a common solution for abandoned children in Eastern Europe, the Middle East, as well as from African and Asian countries. Although IC is rarely observed in North America and Western Europe, that is where the overwhelming majority of scientific research on the role of IC in human well-being is taking place. Therefore, most of the data regarding children growing up without their biological parents is derived from studies investigating the effects of foster/adoption family transition - the most common solution for orphans or abandoned children in North American and Western European countries - on children's developmental outcomes (e.g., Dubois-Comtois et al., 2015; Ferrari, Vezzali, & Rosnati, 2017; Piermattei, Pace, Tambelli, D'Onofrio, & Di Folco, 2017).
A Canadian study investigated adults with a history of IC placement, i.e., the physical and mental health of adults in psychiatric institutions who were raised in orphanages (Sigal, Perry, Rossignol, & Ouimet, 2003). The study included men and women ranging from 43 to 73 years of age, who were placed in an institution at an early age (mean age at institutional placement was 5 years, however the majority of respondents were institutionalized right after birth). The indicators of their physical and mental health were compared to a randomly selected representative sample of Quebec residents. The results showed that adults with an IC history had lower educational attainment and marriage rate than their community peers and, in general, lived more isolated lives. These adults, in comparison to the general population, reported chronic stress-related diseases more often (e.g., depression, allergies), however, there were no actual differences in the rates of chronic illness. Yet, although psychological distress and suicidal thoughts (and attempts) were more frequently observed in adults with an IC history, no statistical differences in self-perceived quality of life were identified.
Another study, conducted in Germany, aimed at understanding the relationship between the conditions of adolescence (foster care vs institutional placement) and attachment patterns in adulthood (Nowacki & Schoelmerich, 2010). Results showed that adults raised in foster families demonstrated more secure attachment patterns than adults raised in institutions. In addition, the study illustrated that adults with an IC history had elevated and close to clinical threshold indicators of psychological stress.
There is also some data on the long-term effects of institutionalization. Specifically, data was collected from 100 girls who were internationally adopted from institutions in Hong Kong to Great Britain in the 1960s (Rushton, Grant, Feast, & Simmonds, 2013). All of the girls were institutionalized prior to adoption for an average of 3 months. Data was collected when the women were 42-53 (M = 48) years of age. Comparison groups from the community as well as women who were born and adopted within Great Britain were studied. No statistical differences were found between the groups in their rates of mental health difficulties, marriage and divorce rates, and emotional support from others. The authors of the study speculated that if the institutional experience is long-term, but interrupted by adequate care, the IC experience per se does not elevate the risk for mental and physical health issues in adulthood. These findings have been interpreted as evidence of the potential longitudinal remediation of IC effects during the course of life.
In the English Romanian Adoptee Study (ERA), where the residual effects of IC history in internationally adopted Romanian orphans were investigated, the higher prevalence of attention deficits and hyperactivity in adolescence with an IC history was identified (Kennedy et al., 2016; Rutter et al., 2010). Participants were divided into two groups: individuals who had experienced short-term institutionalization, and those who had experienced long-term deprivation (children adopted by British families right after birth or who had lived in Romanian orphanages less than 6 months, and children who had been residing in Romanian institutions from 6 to 43 months). Data was collected from the participants at two age points: at 15, then 22-25 years of age. Results showed that the institutionalization effect was more pronounced for hyperactivity and attention deficits during adolescence (i.e., at the age of 15), however, in individuals who had experienced long-term institutionalization, these indicators were critical at both time points. In sum, it has been shown that institutional care has detrimental long-lasting effects on specific aspects of cognitive and social-emotional development lasting into adulthood.
In 2017, a large Australian study was published (Fernandez et al., 2017) that included 669 adults ranging from 27 to 100 years who had experienced IC. Most of the participants (~ 60%) had accommodation problems in adulthood (were homeless or resided in shelters), and more than 1/3 reported having criminal records. More than 50% of participants had problems coping with stress, or reported having mental health problems and relationship issues.
Based on the empirical data reported above, it can be concluded that a history of IC may have long-lasting effects, contributing to higher stress levels and somatic problems, as well as difficulty with attention and hyperactivity. Yet, other data suggests that an IC history can be remediated over the course of life and does not have to leave a traumatic footprint on development. Given such mixed results, it is evident that further empirical data is required in order to learn more about the residual effects of institutionalization.
It is also noteworthy that apart from the social adaptation issues present in adults with an IC history, there is a societal bias in attitudes toward adults with an IC history. According to sociological surveys, the mass media in Russia has created two positive and two negative images of people with IC histories that are very simplified and stereotypical (Abramov & Antonova, 2017): “the positive independent type” (the self-made character); “positive passive” (lacking a sense of agency), “negative independent” (a deviant trouble maker) and “negative passive” (socially stigmatized) (Ibid., p. 421). This study shows that the “passive” stereotypes are mostly spread and perpetuated by the mass media, whereas the “independent” types are rarely discussed. This tendency can have negative effects on potential employers, teachers, and romantic partners, who as a result develop a bias towards people with an IC history.
In sum, the literature on the long-term impact of IC is both limited in size and inconsistent in findings; clearly, more research is needed to increase its volume and resolve its inconsistencies. This article presents pilot data from a study aimed at assessing the psychological, social and emotional well-being of people with a history of institutionalization compared to a group of peers raised in biological families.
Sample. The sample consisted of 58 participants ranging from 16 to 37 years of age (Med = 20, M = 21.81, SD = 5.77):
People with a history of institutionalization (institutional care, IC): 27 respondents (8 females and 19 males);
Control group of individuals raised in biological families (biological families, BF): 31 participants (13 females and 18 males).
The IC group was recruited in colleges of secondary education and vocational training schools with a structural unit for children left without parental care, as well as through cooperation with charitable foundations involved in helping orphanage graduates. Leaflets with an invitation to participate in the study were distributed in those organizations. All participants were pre-screened for neurological diagnoses and sensory deficits. Only those participants who reported no diagnoses or had corrected sensory deficits (i.e., were wearing glasses) were included in the study. The duration of institutionalization differed within the group and ranged from 2 to 18 years (Med = 10, M = 9.33, SD = 4.61). The control group (BF) was recruited using the same protocol in the same educational institutions as well as through announcements on social media in the Internet. The BF group was recruited to match the IC group on the level of education. Most of the participants in both groups had secondary education (9 grades -- 17 people: 12 in the IC and 5 in the BF), vocational training or were high school graduates (10-11 grades - 34 people: 14 in the IC and 20 in the BF). In the BF group, 2 people had less than 9 grades of education and 4 were college students or graduates. In the IC group 1 participant had a PhD degree. Along with data on their education levels, demographic data regarding marital status, children and accommodation (i.e., living situation, private versus communal) was also collected.
Procedure and methods. In order to evaluate the psychological, social and emotional well-being of the participants, two self-report questionnaires were used: The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) and the World Health Organization Quality of Life Questionnaire (WHOQOL-BREF).
We used the Russian version of the ASEBA obtained from the official distributor of the Achenbach questionnaire in Russia (H. Slobodskaya1). The ASEBA contains a list of questions aimed at evaluating adaptive behavior, social functioning, and maladaptive behavior patterns (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003). Clinical syndromes measured by the ASEBA align with the DSM-4 (Achenbach, Dumenci, & Rescorla, 2003; Kornilova, Grigorenko, & Smirnov, 2005). This is one of the most reliable and comprehensive instruments that measures psychological and social well-being and has been previously used with a subpopulation of teenagers with IC histories (e.g., Escobar, Pereira, & Santelices, 2014; Roskam et al., 2014; Surugiu & Mo^oiu, 2013).
The ASEBA scales include: (1) Adaptive functioning: the Adaptive Functioning Profile that includes items capturing levels of competence in dealing with Friends, Spouse/Partner, Family, Job, and Education; (2) Clinical scales: Anxious/Depressed, Withdrawn, Somatic Complaints, Thought Problems, Attention Problems, Aggressive Behavior, Rule-Breaking Behavior, and Intrusive Behavior; (3) Substance use scale. The reliability of the ASEBA scales for the Russian version was verified as part of its cross-cultural validation (Ivanova et al., 2015). Confirmatory factor analysis showed that the original factor structure of the questionnaire was replicated in the Russian sample (RAMSEA = 0.027, CFI = 0.881, TLI = 0.878, Median loading = 0.60).
The WHOQL-BREF is a self-report form developed by the World Health Organization to evaluate self-perceived life quality that can be compared cross-culturally (WHOQOL Group, 1998). It contains the following scales (with internal consistency for the Russian sample shown by Cronbach's in a multicultural study): Physical Health (0.83), Psychological Health (0.79), Social Relationships (0.74), and Environment (0.80) (Skevington, Lotfy, & O'Connell, 2004).
Prior to data analysis, the ASEBA raw scores were transformed into standardized T-scores (M = 50, SD = 10) as suggested by the ASEBA Manual (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003) using mean and standard deviation of the control group raw scores as a reference. The WHOQL-BREF raw scores were transformed into standardized WHOQL scores using WHOQL Manual (Murphy, Herrman, Hawthorne, Pinzone, & Evert, 2000).
Results. In the statistical analysis we used data from participants with complete secondary education to avoid differences linked with the education level. The final sample included 51 participants: the IC group - 26 people (7 females and 19 males), the BF group - 25 people (9 females and 16 males). No significant differences in terms of age distribution between the two groups were detected (t(df) = 0.020 (47.577), p = 0.984)), nor in the gender distribution (j2(df) = 0.157(1), p = 0.692).
The analysis was aimed at comparing the IC and BF groups based on the ASEBA and WHOQL-BREF questionnaires. We used Student's t-test to compare mean group values.
Considering that 19 variables were compared, the Holm method of multiple comparisons adjustment was applied. Analysis was carried out using R.
The analyses yielded no statistically significant group differences in the ASEBA questionnaire, nor in the WHOQL-BREF (see Table 1). Figure 1 and 2 present distributions for all of the variables of interest among the IC and BF groups.
In order to get more detailed results about the characteristics of the psychological, social and emotional well-being of adults with IC histories, we used a classification tree method (James, Witten, Hastie, & Tibshirani, 2013). This method helps to create a step-by-step classification based on predictors, and to attribute observations to given classes (the dependent variable). In our study the parameters of classification were indicators of psychological, social and emotional well-being, and the predicted class -- the group variable (whether the subject belonged to the IC or BF group). To summarize, the aim of the analysis was to find a configuration of test variables that would accurately predict which group a participant belongs to.
Table 1 Results of the Group Comparison
Scale |
IC: M (SD) |
BF: M (SD) |
f(df) |
P |
adj. p |
Cohen's d |
|
ASEBA scales |
|||||||
Adaptiveness |
47.69 (6.98) |
50.04 (7.02) |
-1.20 (49) |
0.237 |
1.000 |
0.336 |
|
Substance Use |
51.52 (7.67) |
49.87 (6.02) |
0.82 (46) |
0.414 |
1.000 |
0.293 |
|
Anxious/Depressed |
45.54 (6.92) |
49.34 (9.88) |
-1.53 (46) |
0.133 |
1.000 |
0.446 |
|
Withdrawn |
50.69 (8.11) |
48.66 (9.64) |
0.79 (47) |
0.431 |
1.000 |
0.228 |
|
Somatic Complaints |
44.59 (8.54) |
48.04 (8.98) |
-1.36 (46) |
0.180 |
1.000 |
0.394 |
|
Thought Problems |
45.42 (7.08) |
49.36 (10.61) |
-1.52 (47) |
0.134 |
1.000 |
0.437 |
|
Attention Problems |
47.58 (9.07) |
49.50 (10.28) |
-0.69 (47) |
0.493 |
1.000 |
0.198 |
|
Aggressive Behavior |
45.42 (6.77) |
48.40 (9.21) |
-1.29 (47) |
0.204 |
1.000 |
0.369 |
|
Rule-Breaking Behavior |
46.98 (8.03) |
49.64 (10.8) |
-0.98 (47) |
0.334 |
1.000 |
0.280 |
|
Intrusive |
45.33 (9.89) |
49.96 (9.97) |
-1.67 (49) |
0.102 |
1.000 |
0.466 |
|
Other Problems |
41.80 (8.55) |
49.58 (10.07) |
-2.87 (46) |
0.006 |
0.117 |
0.833 |
|
Internalization |
46.04 (7.83) |
48.66 (9.64) |
-1.03 (46) |
0.310 |
1.000 |
0.298 |
|
Externalization |
45.35 (8.18) |
49.08 (10.08) |
-1.42 (47) |
0.163 |
1.000 |
0.406 |
|
Additional Problems |
43.96 (8.35) |
49.34 (10.55) |
-1.95 (46) |
0.057 |
1.000 |
0.565 |
|
Total Problems Score |
44.72 (7.92) |
48.94 (10.13) |
-1.60 (46) |
0.117 |
1.000 |
0.464 |
|
WHOQL-BREF |
|||||||
Physical Health |
15.62 (1.58) |
16.20 (1.96) |
-1.18 (49) |
0.245 |
1.000 |
0.326 |
|
Psychological Health |
14.38 (2.35) |
15.40 (1.98) |
-1.67 (49) |
0.102 |
1.000 |
0.469 |
|
Social Relationships |
14.42 (3.64) |
14.84 (2.79) |
-0.46 (49) |
0.649 |
1.000 |
0.130 |
|
Environment |
13.81 (2.47) |
14.32 (2.41) |
-0.75 (49) |
0.457 |
1.000 |
0.209 |
* ASEBA scale means for the BF group diverge from 50 (mean for T-scores) due the exclusion of 6 BF participants from the statistical analysis
We considered primary values from the ASEBA questionnaire as potential predictors of group membership (IC or BF). As additional potential predictors we used demographic data about marital status, children, and accommodation, as they reflect the actual life status of study participants and can impact the questionnaire data. Only full cases where all indicator-predictor data was available were used for the classification - 46 cases in total (23 in each group).
Figure 1
Social and Emotional Well-Being in the IC and BF Groups' ASEBA Scales
Figure 2
Social and Emotional Well-Being in the IC and BF Groups' WHOQL-BREF Scales
The analysis was carried out in R using a tree package: Classification and Regression Trees (Ripley, 2016). The resulting classification tree had a low error rate of group belonging identification - 9% (4 cases out of 46 were incorrectly classified: 2 subject from the BF group and 2 subjects in the IC group). The following predictors were used for the classification: Accommodation, Withdrawn, Attention problems, Environment and Physical Health. The results of the classification are presented in Figure 3.
Figure 3 Classification Results
As evident in Figure 3, the primary parameter for classification was the participant's accommodation. Based on this classification, people with an IC history can be characterized according to the following (compared to control group):
- When living in a private apartment, people with an IC history demonstrate higher levels of Withdrawn, and rate environmental well-being higher than the BF group;
- When living in communal apartments or dorms, people with an IC history
• Either demonstrate lower levels of Attention problems than subjects in the BF group,
• Or do not differ from the BF group in the rate of Attention problems, but report lower physical health compared to peers in the control group.
Note. The number in brackets corresponds to the number of participants in the subgroup; * indicates errors in classification: BF (6/1)* means that the model correctly classifies 6 subjects in the BF group and 1 in the IC group.
Discussion
We did not find any significant differences in the indicators of psychological, social and emotional well-being between adults with a history of institutionalization and peers from the control group raised in biological families. Although preliminary, our results suggest that the social adaptation of people with an IC history is more successful than their cognitive development and language development. Our results are in agreement with the study that demonstrated no statistically significant differences in indicators of mental and physical health between adults with an IC history and those without it (Rushton et al., 2013).
We demonstrated that the social adaption of adults with an IC history is characterized by lower scores on all scales except Substance Use and Withdrawn (in terms of ASEBA scales). In other words, adults who were raised in biological families seem to have poorer adaptive skills than people with an IC history who graduated from an orphanage. This finding leads us to three contradicting hypotheses that need further investigation.
Hypothesis 1. These findings are reliable and there are no statistically significant group differences in the psychological, social and emotional well-being between adults with and without a history of institutionalization. This finding needs replication on a larger sample, which is currently being recruited.
Hypothesis 2. The absence of group differences may be attributed to the lack of self-reflection in adults with an IC history that is indirectly supported by significantly lower IQ scores in this group compared to a control group (based on the CFIT). We suggest the following mediating mechanism: people with lower IQs have less awareness regarding their behavior patterns that yields lower problem behavior scores on the ASEBA. This hypothesis contradicts results obtained in the
Canadian study (Sigal et al., 2003) of higher rates of self-reported chronic stress- related diseases in adults with a history of IC placement. Nevertheless we assume that cognitive deficits might affect performance on standardized written questionnaires.
Hypothesis 3. Given their life history, adults with a history of institutionalization might deliberately report fewer problem behaviors than their age-peers in order to present a more socially acceptable profile.
Thus, in formulating such hypotheses it is important to consider the results of the classification procedure conducted during this pilot study. Findings show that accommodation is an important classification parameter: a private apartment or a communal space - a communal apartment or a dorm. This finding suggests an additional hypothesis about the impact of the current environment on psychological, social and emotional well-being. Adults with a history of institutionalization who live in private apartments are exposed to less familiar (albeit comfortable) conditions. This subgroup demonstrates higher levels of social isolation than adults raised in biological families. This can be explained by the possible lack of experience in building social networks in adults with an IC history, as most of their social activities were shared only with peers living in the same institution.
As for the adults with an IC history who live in communal apartments or dorms, an important classification indicator was the Attention Problems indicator, with equal or lower scores compared to adults without an IC history. We suggest that this differences can be explained by the fact that communal living is a familiar accommodation for people with an IC history, whereas for adults raised in biological families, cohabiting with peers in communal apartments might be frustrating and stressful, which may lead to attention problems in the control group.
Compared to previously published results about the transition of orphanage graduates to adulthood (Fernandez et al., 2017), we need to emphasize that all our IC participants were recruited in a large city through announcements in different organizations and social networks on the Internet. So our participants represent a group that had opportunities to receive government and charity support. Moreover, most of them were college students using accommodation benefits provided by the educational institution. Thus they did not report any difficulties with their accommodations, unlike the participants in earlier research (Ibid.).
In conclusion, our findings suggest that there is no evidence of lower psychological, social and emotional well-being of adults with a history of institutionalization. Likely, the occurrence of these indicators is linked to history of institutionalization as well as to current life circumstances. We are currently continuing to collect data to better characterize and understand social adaptation and the psychological problems of adults with an IC history.
We are grateful to Matilda Marie Steele of the University of Durham, UK and Mei Tan of the University of Houston, USA for their editorial help.
References
Abramov, R. N., & Antonova, K. A. (2017). Social adaptation among graduates from orphanages in the Russian media: A thematic analysis of publications for 2014-2015. Zhurnal Issledovanii Sotsial'noi Politiki, 15(3), 421-434. (in Russian)
Achenbach, T. M., Dumenci, L., & Rescorla, L. (2003). Ratings of relations between DSM-IVdiagnostic categories and items of the Adult Self-Report (ASR) and Adult Behavior Checklist (ABCL). Retrieved from http://www.aseba.com/research/dsm-adultratings.pdf
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Manual for the ASEBA Adult Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.
Bakhmatova, T. G., & Chusova, Y. A. (2013). Analysis of orphans' exclusion on labour market. Baikal Research Journal, 3, 24. Retrieved from http://brj-bguep.ru/reader/article.aspx?id=18121 (in Russian)
Bobyleva, I. A. (2007). Sotsialnaya adaptatsiya vypusknikov internatnykh uchrezhdenii [Social adaptation of graduates of orphanage institutions]. Moscow: Natsional'nyi fond zashchity detei ot zhestokogo obrashcheniya. (in Russian)
Bos, K., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., Drury, S. S., McLaughlin, K. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2011). Psychiatric outcomes in young children with a history of institutionalization. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 19(1), 15-24. doi:10.3109/10673229.2011.549773
Dubois-Comtois, K., Bernier, A., Tarabulsy, G. M., Cyr, C., St-Laurent, D., Lanctot, A. S Beliveau, M. J. (2015). Behavior problems of children in foster care: Associations with foster mothers' representations, commitment, and the quality of mother-child interaction. Child Abuse and Neglect, 48, 119-130. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.06.009
Erol, N., Oztop, D., & Ozcan, O. O. (2008). [Epidemiology of emotional and behavioral problems in children and adolescents reared in orphanages: a national comparative study]. Turk Psikiyatri Dergisi [Turkish Journal of Psychiatry], 19(3), 235-246. (in Turkish)
Escobar, M. J., Pereira, X., & Santelices, M. P. (2014). Behavior problems and attachment in adopted and non-adopted adolescents. Children and Youth Services Review, 42, 59-66.
Fernandez, E., Lee, J.-S., Foote, W., Blunden, H., McNamara, P., Kovacs, S., Cornefert, P.-A. (2017). `There's more to be done; “Sorry” is just a word': Legacies of out-of-home care in the 20th century. Children Australia, 42(3), 176-197.
Ferrari, L., Vezzali, L., & Rosnati, R. (2017). The role of adoptive parents' intergroup contact in fostering the well-being of adoptees: The “extended intragroup contact effect”. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 59, 43-52. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2017.04.014
Groze, V., & Ileana, D. (1996). A follow-up study of adopted children from Romania. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 13(6), 541-565.
Ivanova, M. Y., Achenbach, T. M., Rescorla, L. A., Turner, L. V., Ahmeti-Pronaj, A., Au, A. Chen, Y.-C. (2015). Syndromes of self-reported psychopathology for ages 18-59 in 29 societies. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 37(2), 171-183.
James, G., Witten, D., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2013). An introduction to statistical learning. New York: Springer.
Kennedy, M., Kreppner, J., Knights, N., Kumsta, R., Maughan, B., Golm, D. Sonuga Barke, E. J. (2016).
Early severe institutional deprivation is associated with a persistent variant of adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: clinical presentation, developmental continuities and life circumstances in the English and Romanian adoptees study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(10), 1113-1125.
Kornilova, T. V., Grigorenko, E. L., & Smirnov, S. D. (2005). Podrostkigrupp riska [Adolescents at risk]. Saint Petersburg: Piter. (in Russian)
Merz, E. C., & McCall, R. B. (2010). Behavior problems in children adopted from psychosocially depriving institutions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(4), 459-470.
Murphy, B., Herrman, H., Hawthorne, G., Pinzone, T., & Evert, H. (2000). The World Health Organization Quality of Life (WHOQOL) Study: Australian WHOQOL-100, WHOQOL-BREF and CA-WHOQOL instruments user's manual and interpretation guide. Melbourne: WHOQOL Field Study Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne and St Vincent's Mental Health Service.
Nowacki, K., & Schoelmerich, A. (2010). Growing up in foster families or institutions: Attachment representation and psychological adjustment of young adults. Attachment and Human Development, 12(6), 551-566.
Pastukhova, D. A. (2011). Experimental study of psychological adaptation of preschool and primary school age orphans. Vestnik Buryatskogo gosudarstvennogo universiteta [Bulletin of the Buryat State University], 5, 206-210. (in Russian)
Piermattei, C., Pace, C. S., Tambelli, R., D'Onofrio, E., & Di Folco, S. (2017). Late adoptions: Attachment security and emotional availability in mother-child and father-child dyads. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(8), 2114-2125. doi:10.1007/s10826-017-0732-6
Prisyazhnaya, N. V. (2007). Orphan children: Entering life after graduation. Sotsiologicheskie Issledovaniya, 11, 54-63. (in Russian)
Ripley, B. (2016). tree: Classification and Regression Trees. Retrieved from https://cran.r- project.org/web/packages/tree/index.html
Roskam, I., Stievenart, M., Tessier, R., Muntean, A., Escobar, M. J., Santelices, M. Pierrehumbert, B. (2014). Another way of thinking about ADHD: the predictive role of early attachment deprivation in adolescents' level of symptoms. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(1), 133-144.
Rushton, A., Grant, M., Feast, J., & Simmonds, J. (2013). The British Chinese Adoption Study: Orphanage care, adoption and midlife outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(11), 1215-1222.
Rutter, M., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Kreppner, J., Kumsta, R., ... Gunnar, M. R. (2010). Deprivation-specific psychological patterns: Effects of institutional deprivation. Boston, MA/Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
Sem'ya, G. V. (2007). Rossiyskiy opyt raboty s vypusknikami internatnykh uchrezhdeniy [The Russian experience with graduates of orphanage institutions]. Nevolya, 14. Retrieved from http://www.index.org.ru/nevol/2007-14/15_semya_n14.html
Shulga, T. I. (2011). Social and psychological problems of orphanage graduates and children left without parental care. Vestnik Moskovskogo gosudarstvennogo oblastnogo universiteta: elektronnyi jurnal [Bulletin of Moscow State Regional University: Electronic Journal], 4, 85-95. (in Russian)
Sigal, J. J., Perry, J. C., Rossignol, M., & Ouimet, M. C. (2003). Unwanted infants: Psychological and physical consequences of inadequate orphanage care 50 years later. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 73(1), 3-12.
Skevington, S. M., Lotfy, M., & O'Connell, K. A. (2004). The World Health Organization's WHO- QOL-BREF quality of life assessment: psychometric properties and results of the international field trial. A report from the WHOQOL group. Quality of Life Research, 13(2), 299-310. Surugiu, S. I., & Mo^oiu, C. (2013). The aggressive behavior of adolescents in institutionalized system.
Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 78, 546-550.
WHOQOL Group. (1998). Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. Psychological Medicine, 28(3), 551-558.
Размещено на Allbest.ru
...Подобные документы
The problem of evaluation, self-assessment of personality as a psychological category. Factors of formation evaluation and self-esteem of children of primary school age. An experimental study of characteristics evaluation and self-esteem of junior pupils.
курсовая работа [28,6 K], добавлен 19.05.2011The definition of conformism as passive acceptance and adaptation to standards of personal conduct, rules and regulations of the cult of absolute power. Study the phenomenon of group pressure. External and internal views of subordination to the group.
реферат [15,3 K], добавлен 14.05.2011Theoretical basis of a role plays as a teaching aid. Historic background of game origin. Psychological value of a role plays. The main function and principles of game organization. Gaming technique. Classification of role plays. Advantages of a game.
курсовая работа [50,7 K], добавлен 26.04.2013The study of harm to children from watching American cartoons. Problem of imitating negative or mindless characters from cartoons. Leading role of American cartoon industry in the animation history. First steps in the progress of a child’s development.
эссе [16,3 K], добавлен 11.04.2013Основные направления прикладных исследований в социальной психологии. Сравнительная характеристика структурных компонентов программ социально-психологического и социологического исследования. Сценарий фокус-групп по проблеме "Насилие в семье над детьми".
контрольная работа [31,8 K], добавлен 08.10.2012Studies by Fischer and his colleagues and Dawson (2006) have investigated development in a wide range of domains, including understanding of social interaction concepts such as "nice" and "mean", skills in mathematics, and understanding "leadership".
реферат [20,2 K], добавлен 22.12.2009Definition of Leadership. Trait theory. How this theory works. Origin and Analysis and basics Pre-conditions for effective use of Trait theory. Inborn leadership characteristics. Process of impact and interaction among the leader and his followers.
реферат [436,9 K], добавлен 24.09.2014Unhealthy food, lack of sleep, passive lifestyle, related works. Survey, Passive Lifestyle, Lack Of Sleep, Nutrition. How often pupils have negative feelings. Teachers' complaints. Can we do to reduce these negative displays of pupil’s behavior.
курсовая работа [25,5 K], добавлен 18.05.2015Influence psychology of cognitive activity and cognitive development on student’s learning abilities during study. Cognitive development theory in psychology. Analysis of Jean Piaget's theory. Her place among the other concept of personal development.
презентация [1,3 M], добавлен 13.04.2016The events in the novel "To Kill a Mockingbird". The opposition between children’s and adults. "The Secret Diary of Adrian Mole" as the picture of the world from the point of view of a teenager. Examples of Adrian’s relations with adults in the novel.
реферат [13,5 K], добавлен 16.05.2016Principles of learning and language learning. Components of communicative competence. Differences between children and adults in language learning. The Direct Method as an important method of teaching speaking. Giving motivation to learn a language.
курсовая работа [66,2 K], добавлен 22.12.2011The necessity of using innovative social technologies and exploring the concept of social entrepreneurship. Analyzes current level of development of social entrepreneurship in Ukraine, the existing problems of creating favorable organizational.
статья [54,5 K], добавлен 19.09.2017General characteristic of the LLC DTEK Zuevskaya TPP and its main function. The history of appearance and development of the company. Characteristics of the organizational management structure. Analysis of financial and economic performance indicators.
отчет по практике [4,2 M], добавлен 22.05.2015Planning a research study. Explanation, as an ability to give a good theoretical background of the problem, foresee what can happen later and introduce a way of solution. Identifying a significant research problem. Conducting a pilot and the main study.
реферат [26,5 K], добавлен 01.04.2012Detection the benefits of Corporate Social Responsibility strategies that would serve as a motivation for managers and shareholders in the context of a classical firm, which possesses monetary preferences. Theoretical framework and hypothesis development.
курсовая работа [319,5 K], добавлен 14.02.2016Factors threatening the environment. Habitat destruction and species extinction. Depletion of the ozone layer. The living portion of an ecosystem. The environment in the new millennium: the way of the world. The crisis of ecology in the developing world.
статья [47,8 K], добавлен 21.11.2009Present-day issues of foreign language teaching at secondary school. Current concepts in secondary school graduates EFL. Psychological analysis of Gardner's Theory. Learning environment in teaching English conversation.
дипломная работа [71,5 K], добавлен 20.11.2004Teaching practice is an important and exciting step in the study of language. Description of extracurricular activities. Feedback of extracurricular activity. Psychological characteristic of a group and a students. Evaluation and testing of students.
отчет по практике [87,0 K], добавлен 20.02.2013The definition of Corporate Social Responsibility and main approaches. Stakeholder VS Shareholders. The principles of CSR: features and problems. Sanofi Group Company and its Social Responsibility program. Results and Perspectives, the global need.
курсовая работа [43,2 K], добавлен 09.03.2015Four common social classes. Karl Marx's social theory of class. Analysis the nature of class relations. The conflict as the key driving force of history and the main determinant of social trajectories. Today’s social classes. Postindustrial societies.
презентация [718,4 K], добавлен 05.04.2014